Background

Hypoxemic episodes occur during sleep after abdominal surgery, possibly caused by airway obstruction. The authors found arousals from sleep more often than respiratory disturbances, so they related changes in sleep state (short arousals from sleep and longer periods of wakening) to the sudden increase in respiratory flow that indicates relief from complete or partial respiratory obstruction.

Methods

Sleep state and nasal flow were studied in 16 patients receiving patient-controlled morphine and oxygen by facemask on the night after routine gynecologic surgery. Traces were analyzed separately for sleep events and for sudden increases in respiratory flow. The authors noted sleep events (arousals from sleep and transition from sleep to wake) that occurred within 12 s of relief of obstruction.

Results

Sleep quality was poor, with only stage 2 sleep in most patients. Median sleep duration was 70% of the study period, with 15 arousals and 6 awakenings per hour of sleep. Only 30% of arousals and awakenings were associated with relief of obstruction. Relief of obstruction also occurred without arousal from sleep, with a median frequency of 38 (30-62) in each night. Relief of obstruction was more frequently associated with arousal from sleep after benzodiazepine premedication (33% vs. 28%; P = 0.012), but this allocation was not randomized.

Conclusions

Arousals from sleep are frequent after abdominal surgery and mostly not related to respiratory disturbance. Relief of respiratory obstruction can occur during sleep without sleep arousal and during wakefulness.

SLEEP is often disturbed in patients recovering from major 1and minor surgery, 2but disturbance is less when opioid analgesia is not used. 3Opioid analgesics may cause sleep disturbance, 4and this sleep disturbance is associated with impaired oxygenation. 5,6 

In sleep apnea syndrome, episodes of airway obstruction and hypoxemia impair daytime function 7and may have harmful cardiovascular effects. 8–10It is tempting to suppose that similar associations are present after surgery because patients sleep poorly after surgery, have airway obstruction, and have repeated arousals from sleep and episodes of hypoxemia. 5,11An arousal from sleep is defined as a short increase in electroencephalographic frequency, from a period of at least 10 s of sleep, that does not necessarily lead to a longer period of wakefulness. However, the frequency of arousals is greater than the frequency of episodic obstruction. 12Although there is no doubt that changes in sleep state are associated with changes in respiration 13,14and that repeated changes in sleep state affect respiratory reflexes, 15–17the explanation that arousals from sleep are caused by episodes of airway obstruction does not fit our observations adequately. To find how sleep and respiratory changes are linked, we studied patients receiving morphine via  patient-controlled administration. We noted the episodes of relief of partial obstruction, arousals from sleep, and changes of sleep state. We chose relief of obstruction as an index of respiratory disturbance because it is an abrupt respiratory change that can be recognized easily and exactly timed to within a breath or two. In contrast, the onset of partial respiratory obstruction is often gradual and difficult to define. We considered that changes in respiration were linked with sleep events if they decreased within 12 s of a change in electroencephalographic state. Ventilatory changes occur 4–8 s after changes in sleep, 13,14so we used twice this time difference to include most breathing changes that could be related to a change in electroencephalographic state.

This article is featured in “This Month in Anesthesiology.” Please see this issue of Anesthesiology, page 5A.

With approval from the Lothian Health Board Ethics Committee (Edinburgh, United Kingdom), we observed patients after surgery. We did not change clinical management other than to apply monitoring equipment. We asked women who were about to have abdominal surgery for nonmalignant gynecologic conditions to participate, and we obtained written informed consent. We excluded patients who were already taking opioid analgesic agents or other centrally acting medication, such as antidepressants, anxiolytics, and night sedatives. All patients came to the hospital on the morning of surgery. Because this was an observational study, anesthetic management was not standardized and was given according to clinical routine. However, management was similar in all cases: either no premedication or premedication with temazepam or diazepam by mouth between 1 and 2 h before surgery, followed by propofol intravenous for anesthetic induction, nitrous oxide, and isoflurane to maintain anesthesia, and neuromuscular blockade with either atracurium or vecuronium. Opioid supplements during surgery were given either as morphine or, in one case, diamorphine. Some patients were given nonsteroidal analgesic agents, such as diclofenac rectally or piroxicam sublingually. After surgery, all patients were given patient-controlled analgesia (2 mg intravenous morphine with a 5-min lockout period between demands). All patients received prophylactic antiemetic treatment with 50 mg cyclizine intravenous at the end of surgery and 2.5 mg cyclizine with each morphine dose.

During anesthesia and surgery, the following electrodes and sensors were applied. For electroencephalogram recording, we inserted subcutaneous scalp electrodes in the C3–C4, Cz–Pz positions and fastened them in place with collodion. We applied disposable gel electrodes above and lateral to the eyes for electrooculography and submentally for electromyographic measurement.

After the patient had returned to the ward after the operation, we taped a pulse oximeter probe to a finger (Embla; Flaga hf, Reykjavik, Iceland) and placed a nasal pressure cannula (Pro-Tech airflow sensor; Woodinville, WA), similar to a very narrow nasal oxygen cannula, in the nostrils. This cannula was connected to a differential pressure transducer (type 3EA; Gaeltec, Skye, United Kingdom) and generates a signal that is proportional to nasal flow. 18,19The devices were taped firmly in place, and the cables and tubing were lead in a single bundle from the patient's shoulder to a miniature data logging harness. The patients recovered from anesthesia in a dedicated recovery area where they were given intravenous morphine until comfortable, and patient-controlled analgesia was established. They were then nursed in a large gynecologic ward containing 20 beds. When the patient was stable and comfortable in the ward, we connected a miniature data-logging system (Embla) set to start recording from 10 pm until 6 am the next morning. The submental electromyogram was filtered between 22 and 90 Hz with a 50-Hz notch, and other signals were filtered between 0.3 and 35 Hz.

No special precautions were taken to ensure that the ward was quiet or to adjust the level of lighting. However, the usual ward routine is to dim the lights between 22:00 and 06:00 h, and ward activity between these times is limited. Oxygen therapy using a simple variable function mask with a flow of 4 l/min is standard for our patients receiving patient-controlled analgesia, and this was given over the study period. Patients were not monitored clinically for pulse oxygen saturation, and the measurements that we recorded were not displayed. The next morning, the recording apparatus was removed, and the patients were asked about their quality of sleep, using semistructured questions.

The signals were transferred to a laboratory display unit (Somnologics; Flaga hf) and coded so that the traces could be studied without knowing their source. The respiratory signal and signals for sleep staging were analyzed separately by a single trained investigator (A. W.). Respiratory and sleep signals were analyzed using the same 30-s epochs. Using the standard scoring system used since 1968, 20sleep was classified into the following states: alert or relaxed wakefulness, non–rapid eye movement (REM) stages 1–4, and REM. Each epoch was considered either wake or sleep if either state occupied more than 15 s. The time of start and the duration of wake periods were noted.

Electroencephalographic arousals were recognized using the method of Cheshire et al. , 21and the times were noted. Using this method, an arousal is at least 1.5 s of alpha or theta electroencephalogram rhythm and a concurrent increase in submental electromyographic activity, after a period of at least 10 s of sleep. The nasal flow signal was scored for events that we classified as relief of obstruction, and the times were noted. A relief of obstruction had occurred if one or more of the following criteria were present:

  1. an obvious increase in respiratory frequency, within three respiratory cycles

  2. a change in the inspiratory flow pattern from a flattened to a more rounded shape 19 

  3. a sudden increase in flow amplitude 22(fig. 1)

Fig. 1. Nasal cannular pressure. An example of recovery from partial respiratory obstruction: At the arrow , there is an abrupt change in signal amplitude, associated with an increase in frequency and a change in waveform. The preceding waveform is flattened.

Fig. 1. Nasal cannular pressure. An example of recovery from partial respiratory obstruction: At the arrow , there is an abrupt change in signal amplitude, associated with an increase in frequency and a change in waveform. The preceding waveform is flattened.

Close modal

Generally, two or three of these features were present. After episodes of relief of obstruction had been identified, we retrospectively noted the features that had been used for identification.

After the signals used for sleep and respiratory events had been separately classified, the code was broken, and the data were exported to a spreadsheet (Excel 2000, version 9.0; Microsoft Corp., Seattle, WA) to determine the times between the electroencephalographic features (arousal or wake) and relief of obstruction. Relief of respiratory obstruction and sleep events were considered related if they occurred within 12 s of each other (fig. 2). They were classified as relief of obstruction in relation to either electroencephalographic arousal (the short-term appearance of the wake state) or transition to wakefulness (when an epoch of sleep is followed by at least 1 min of waking). We noted whether electroencephalographic arousal or transition to wakefulness was either preceded or followed by a relief of obstruction or whether sleep events occurred alone. Oxygenation during the study period was summarized as the mean and SD of oxygen saturation values at 1-s intervals.

Fig. 2. Analysis allowing association between events. Each event (recovery from obstruction, arousal from sleep, and transition from sleep to wake) is coded by a bar  given a different arbitrary amplitude. Sleep state is shown by awake as 5, asleep as 0, a recovery from obstruction as 3, and an arousal as 1. Arrows  indicate the possible separate classifications. (A ) Recovery from obstruction alone; (B ) recovery from obstruction associated with arousal; (C ) arousal alone; (D ) recovery from obstruction and transition from sleep to wake. EEG = electroencep-halographic.

Fig. 2. Analysis allowing association between events. Each event (recovery from obstruction, arousal from sleep, and transition from sleep to wake) is coded by a bar  given a different arbitrary amplitude. Sleep state is shown by awake as 5, asleep as 0, a recovery from obstruction as 3, and an arousal as 1. Arrows  indicate the possible separate classifications. (A ) Recovery from obstruction alone; (B ) recovery from obstruction associated with arousal; (C ) arousal alone; (D ) recovery from obstruction and transition from sleep to wake. EEG = electroencep-halographic.

Close modal

Statistical Management

Data analysis was predominantly descriptive, using median and upper and lower quartile values (Minitab release 12, 1998; Westwood Business Park Coventry, United Kingdom). The frequencies of events were compared with analysis of variance. Post hoc  observations were interpreted with the Mann–Whitney U test.

We asked 23 patients to participate. Four declined, and recordings were technically inadequate in three subjects. Two additional subjects were excluded: one because of a variation in anesthetic technique (epidural analgesia); the other did not have a transverse lower abdominal incision. In one subject, recording of oxygen saturation was not successful, but the other data from this patient were used.

Details of the patients analyzed are given in table 1, separated according to premedication. There was no significant difference in characteristics between those who did and did not receive premedication.

Table 1. Patient Details

PCA = patient-controlled analgesia.

Table 1. Patient Details
Table 1. Patient Details

Generally, the patients did not report good sleep. The median proportion of sleep in the study period from 22:00 to 06:00 h was 70%. No patients had any stage 3 or 4 sleep, and only four patients had any REM sleep, ranging between 8 and 86 min. The characteristics of the sleep for each patient are shown in table 2. There were many arousals (short periods) and awakenings (longer changes) from sleep. The median number of awakenings in the 8-h study period was 35 (22–45), and the frequency of arousals per hour of sleep was 15 (10–20). The number of electroencephalographic events (transient arousal or wakening) associated with relief of obstruction was 36 (20–50) per night. This was significantly less than the total number of arousals and wakenings (78 [55–106]; P < 0.001). A typical example of an arousal from sleep, with no change in respiratory pattern, is shown in figure 3, and an episode of relief from respiratory obstruction, in a conscious subject, is shown in figure 4.

Table 2. Sleep Pattern, Arousals and Awakenings, and Relation with Recovery from Obstruction (Respiratory Events)

EEG = electroencephalographic; Q1 = first quartile value; Q3 = third quartile value; REM = rapid eye movement; Resp after = respiratory event after EEG arousal; Resp first = respiratory event before EEG arousal; Spo2= oxygen saturation measured by pulse oximetry.

Table 2. Sleep Pattern, Arousals and Awakenings, and Relation with Recovery from Obstruction (Respiratory Events)
Table 2. Sleep Pattern, Arousals and Awakenings, and Relation with Recovery from Obstruction (Respiratory Events)

Fig. 3. An example of a short period of arousal from sleep, indicated by the solid bar , with no change in breathing pattern. The traces are two electroencephalogram channels, a submental electromyogram (EMG), and nasal flow.

Fig. 3. An example of a short period of arousal from sleep, indicated by the solid bar , with no change in breathing pattern. The traces are two electroencephalogram channels, a submental electromyogram (EMG), and nasal flow.

Close modal

Fig. 4. An example of relief of obstruction (arrow ), in a patient who was awake. EMG = electromyogram.

Fig. 4. An example of relief of obstruction (arrow ), in a patient who was awake. EMG = electromyogram.

Close modal

An episode of relief from respiratory obstruction generally showed all three of the criteria used (median frequency of all three criteria occurring in these events, for all the subjects, was 53%[34–58%]). A loss of flattening of the waveform was seen in 80% (60–98%), and amplitude changes were seen in 74% (5–100%). Waveform and amplitude changes were associated alone in 10% of events, and waveform and frequency were associated in 14%. In only two subjects were frequency changes seen in isolation.

The number of episodes of relief of respiratory obstruction in the study period not linked with sleep events (either arousals or awakenings) was 38 (30–62). This was greater than the number of occasions when respiratory and sleep events were related (fig. 5). The populations in which recovery from respiratory obstruction was not related to sleep events and the population in which respiratory and sleep events were related were significantly different (analysis of variance, P < 0.001). Sleep was fragmented. The rate of wakening was 4.8 (3.1–7.3) episodes per hour of sleep.

Fig. 5. Distribution of the total events recorded in the patients over the observation period. Associated = occasions when relief from respiratory obstruction and sleep state change occurred within 12 s; EEG = electroencephalogram; EEG only = either arousal or transition to wake, without association with a recovery from obstruction; resp only = relief of respiratory obstruction, not associated with change in sleep state.

Fig. 5. Distribution of the total events recorded in the patients over the observation period. Associated = occasions when relief from respiratory obstruction and sleep state change occurred within 12 s; EEG = electroencephalogram; EEG only = either arousal or transition to wake, without association with a recovery from obstruction; resp only = relief of respiratory obstruction, not associated with change in sleep state.

Close modal

Patients who had received premedication spent less time asleep and had more respiratory-related arousals. After premedication, the proportion of respiratory-related arousals was 32% (31–36%), compared with 28% (22–29%) (P < 0.05;fig. 6). Premedication had no apparent effect on the incidence of respiratory disturbances not associated with arousal.

Fig. 6. Proportion of study time asleep and proportion of arousal and wake events that were related to relief of respiratory obstruction in patients who had received benzodiazepine premedication (filled symbols ) and those who did not receive premedication (open symbols ).

Fig. 6. Proportion of study time asleep and proportion of arousal and wake events that were related to relief of respiratory obstruction in patients who had received benzodiazepine premedication (filled symbols ) and those who did not receive premedication (open symbols ).

Close modal

If sleep events were related to relief of obstruction, this relief of obstruction occurred more frequently before than after the changes in sleep state, but the difference was not great. Premedication did not have a clear effect (fig. 7).

Fig. 7. Relation between arousals from sleep (top ) and transition from sleep to wake (bottom ) with episodes of recovery from obstruction. In most cases (center columns ), there was no association with recovery from obstruction. In fewer cases, relief of respiratory obstruction could precede or follow arousal or awakening (left and right columns ). There was no clear effect of premedication (open columns = no premedication; shaded columns = patients who received benzodiazepine premedication).

Fig. 7. Relation between arousals from sleep (top ) and transition from sleep to wake (bottom ) with episodes of recovery from obstruction. In most cases (center columns ), there was no association with recovery from obstruction. In fewer cases, relief of respiratory obstruction could precede or follow arousal or awakening (left and right columns ). There was no clear effect of premedication (open columns = no premedication; shaded columns = patients who received benzodiazepine premedication).

Close modal

Oxygenation was satisfactory in all except one patient, who had a mean saturation of only 90%. The record showed a period of a lower saturation, later returning to a satisfactory value. The oxygen mask probably came off and was then replaced. In the remainder of the patients, the mean oxygen saturation measured by pulse oximetry (Spo2) was 98%, and the mean SD of the values in each patient was 1.2%, indicating that there were very few changes in Spo2. We found no relations between oxygen saturation and respiratory disturbances, morphine use and the other measurements, or sleep quality and subjective reports from the patients. Most patients reported poor sleep and no dreams, and no patient wished to have measurements for a further night.

In a previous study of patients receiving morphine analgesia after abdominal surgery, we found that hypoxemic events and airway obstruction 23were more frequent when the patients were awake. Perhaps respiratory disturbances or hypoxemia start when the patient falls asleep and then cause arousal, so that the epoch in which they were noted is classified as wakefulness. In this way, an event might seem to have occurred in the awake state, but it could have started when the patient was asleep.

We studied this possibility and sought to clarify the relation between relief of respiratory obstruction and arousals or awakenings. Sleep was generally poor, usually stage 2, with frequent awakenings and even more frequent arousals. Only approximately half of these sleep events were in relation to respiratory disturbances. Although they were less frequent, we also found relief of respiratory obstruction that was not associated with awakenings or arousals, and only 57% of these occurred during sleep. Our findings do not support the possibility that relief of airway obstruction precedes arousal from sleep or that relief of obstruction can be classified as occurring in a period of wakefulness when in fact it may have started during sleep.

The criteria to define arousal  that we used were standard but may not detect small changes in sleep state. For example, playing a sound to a sleeping patient with obstructive sleep apnea can shorten a period of obstruction, even though no electroencephalographic arousal is evident. 24Electroencephalographic frequencies change during the progress of obstructive apneas, irrespective of whether the obstruction ends in an arousal. 25This suggests that arousal is gradual and not a categorical event. Our method of identifying arousal from sleep may not identify lesser degrees of arousal.

Several definitions are used for an arousal. 26The most frequent, defined by the American Sleep Disorders Association, 27is only moderately repeatable, particularly when judging arousal from light sleep. 28We used a well-accepted and relatively stringent definition, which includes an increase in chin electromyogram, however brief, and a return to theta or alpha rhythm for more than 1.5 s. 21A very similar definition (Université Catholique de Louvain, Brussels, Belgium) has comparable agreement and repeatability to the American Sleep Disorders Association definition but is easier to use because of the associated electromyographic changes required. 29 

Other workers have related arousal to changes in respiration, such as obstructive nonapneic events, similar to the majority of the events we studied. For example, esophageal and pharyngeal pressures were measured in patients with mild obstructive sleep apnea to identify respiratory effort–related arousals. 30Most reductions in breathing were associated with cortical “microarousal,” but the definition incorporated both electromyographic and respiratory features.

The onset of partial airway obstruction is associated with a reduction in flow, perhaps diversion of expiratory flow away from the nose, and characteristic changes in the waveform of inspiratory flow. 19Because respiratory flow decreases gradually over a number of breaths, the exact time of onset of these changes in respiration is often not clear-cut. However, recovery from obstruction is characteristically prominent, abrupt, and easy to define. Generally, our patients showed all three criteria of recovery from partial obstruction that we had set. However, a small proportion of patients had respiratory events that were characterized by changes of amplitude and frequency only, with no detectable changes in inspiratory waveform. Because the pattern of distribution of these events was similar in other respects, we consider that these events were biologically similar to the events seen in other patients who had all three features. It is possible that the features of recovery from respiratory obstruction may be characteristic for particular individuals. We are confident that we were detecting recovery from obstructive, nonapneic events and not arousal from sleep that could alter the breathing pattern. However, even if this small proportion of events were misclassified, as they did not have all the features of relief of obstruction, this would not explain the limited association between relief of obstruction and arousal or awakening that we report here.

We looked for a link between these respiratory events and transient arousal or longer-term awakening. Because others have found that respiration and sleep state change either simultaneously 31or within 4–8 s, 13,14,32we took 12 s as a generous time to link changes in respiration and sleep state. We considered the possibilities that either increasing drive with recovery from respiratory obstruction could trigger arousal or arousal could resolve obstruction. In fact, we found that most arousals were independent of recovery from airway obstruction. When recovery from obstruction and arousal were close together, the likelihood of arousal before or after the respiratory event was similar.

In sleep apnea syndrome, the likely mechanism is that arousal leads to recovery from airway obstruction. 33However, our patients did not have this condition. Another factor, such as morphine administration, seems to affect the control of the airway. Previous work comparing morphine analgesia and regional analgesia reported frequent obstructive episodes during morphine administration, but in that study, obstructive episodes were only noted during sleep. 5 

During sleep, upper airway muscle activity is an important factor in the maintenance of airway patency. The neurons that control this muscle tone are controlled by the pontine reticular formation, whose activity is reduced during sleep. The activity of medullary raphe neurons is modulated by a cholinergic mechanism and can be reduced by intrapontine carbachol, 34causing an REM-like state. Serotonergic neurons in the medullary raphe 35tonically activate the hypoglossal nucleus via  5HT2 receptors. Pontine injections of carbachol reduce serotonin release at the hypoglossal neurons 36and reduce genioglossal activity.

Opioids inhibit REM sleep by a μ-opioid receptor mechanism. 37Opioids inhibit acetyl choline release in the medial pontine reticular formation 38,39and cause sleep disruption that can be antagonized by carbachol. 40This sleep disruption is characterized by a decrease in REM sleep. Morphine may dissociate motor control of the upper airway from the sleep state so that airway control may be impaired even in subjects who are awake, leading to loss and return of airway patency such as the episode illustrated in figure 4. In addition, the reduction in respiratory drive by morphine (which is directed to the airway muscles as well as to the diaphragm) may allow loss of airway control of this type even when patients are awake. All the patients we studied also received cyclizine in addition to morphine, in proportion to the morphine doses given by PCA. Cyclizine, like other phenothiazines, can inhibit central cholinergic receptors and cause or accentuate confusion 41and could possibly increase this effect of morphine.

Premedication with benzodiazepines seems to increase the association between respiratory obstruction and sleep arousal. Such an association was not found in a randomized trial of nitrazepam in patients with sleep apnea where hypoxemia or obstructive episodes were not increased. 42However, our patients had undergone surgery and were given opiates, which interact with benzodiazepines, so different results are perhaps likely.

The effects of surgery on sleep have recently been summarized. 43Poor sleep has been reported after surgery 4and in high dependency 44and intensive care units. 45It goes unrecognized by nurses and is found in patients nursed in single rooms and after attempts to keep quiet. 44Sleep disturbance is not known to be harmful, 43and the effects of opioids on sleep quantity and pattern remain to be adequately defined. Although parallels can be drawn between patients after major surgery and patients with the sleep apnea/hypopnea syndrome, there do seem to be some fundamental differences. In the sleep apnea/hypopnea syndrome, the link between increased airway resistance, arousal, and recovery from airway obstruction is recognized and probably causal. After operation, the events of arousal, awakening, and recovery from airway obstruction are not clearly linked. Arousals occur without respiratory changes, and obstruction and recovery from airway obstruction occurs in patients who do not fulfill the standard criteria for sleep.

The authors thank Peter Wraith, Ph.D., and Neil J. Douglas, F.R.C.P.E. (Scottish Sleep Laboratory, Royal Infirmary, Edinburgh, United Kingdom), for their assistance.

1.
Rosenberg-Adamsen S, Kehlet H, Dodds C, Rosenberg J: Postoperative sleep disturbances: Mechanisms and clinical implications. Br J Anaesth 1996; 76: 552–9
2.
Kavey NB, Altshuler KZ: Sleep in herniorrhaphy patients. Am J Surg 1979; 138: 682–7
3.
Rosenberg-Adamsen S, Skarbye M, Wildschiodtz G, Kehlet H, Rosenberg J: Sleep after laparoscopic cholecystectomy. Br J Anaesth 1997; 77: 572–5
4.
Knill RL, Moote CA, Skinner MI, Rose EA: Anesthesia with abdominal surgery leads to intense REM sleep during the first postoperative week. A nesthesiology 1990; 73: 52–61
5.
Catley DM, Thornton C, Jordan C, Lehane JR, Royston D, Jones JG: Pronounced episodic oxygen desaturation in the postoperative period: Its association with ventilatory pattern and analgesic regimen. A nesthesiology 1985; 63: 20–8
6.
Rosenberg J, Wildschiodtz G, Pedersen MH, von Jessen F, Kehlet H: Late postoperative nocturnal episodic hypoxaemia and associated sleep pattern. Br J Anaesth 1994; 72: 145–50
7.
Kingshott RN, Engleman HM, Deary IJ, Douglas NJ: Does arousal frequency predict daytime function? Eur Respir J 1998; 12: 1264–70
8.
Davies RJO, Vardi-Visy K, Clarke M, Stradling JR: Identification of sleep disruption and sleep disordered breathing from the systolic blood pressure profile. Thorax 1993; 48: 1242–7
9.
Davies CWH, Crosby JH, Mullins RL, Barbour C, Davies RJO, Stradling JR: Case-control study of 24 hour ambulatory blood pressure in patients with obstructive sleep apnoea and normal matched control subjects. Thorax 2000; 55: 736–40
10.
Tun Y, Okabe S, Hida W, Kurosawa H, Tabata M, Kikuchi Y, Shirato K: Nocturnal blood pressure during apnoeic and ventilatory periods in patients with obstructive sleep apnoea. Eur Respir J 1999; 14: 1271–7
11.
Rosenberg J, Rasmussen V, Vonjessen F, Ullstad T, Kehlet H: Late postoperative episodic and constant hypoxemia and associated ECG abnormalities. Br J Anaesth 1990; 65: 684–91
12.
Drummond GB, Stedul K, Kingshott R, Rees K, Nimmo AF, Wraith P, Douglas NJ: Automatic CPAP compared with conventional treatment for episodic hypoxemia and sleep disturbance after major abdominal surgery. A nesthesiology 2002; 96: 817–26
13.
Trinder J, Whitworth F, Kay A, Wilkin P: Respiratory instability during sleep onset. J Appl Physiol 1992; 73: 2462–9
14.
Trinder J, VanBeveren JA, Smith P, Kleiman J, Kay A: Correlation between ventilation and EEG-defined arousal during sleep onset in young subjects. J Appl Physiol 1997; 83: 2005–11
15.
Seriès F, Roy N, Marc I: Effects of sleep deprivation and sleep fragmentation on upper airway collapsibility in normal subjects. Am J Resp Crit Care Med 1995; 150: 481–5
16.
Sforza E, Krieger J, Petiau C: Nocturnal evolution of respiratory effort in obstructive sleep apnoea syndrome: Influence on arousal threshold. Eur Respir J 1998; 12: 1257–63
17.
Johnston RV, Grant DA, Wilkinson MH, Walker AM: Repetitive hypoxia rapidly depresses arousal from active sleep in newborn lambs. J Physiol (Lond) 1998; 510: 651–9
18.
Guyatt AR, Parker SP, Mcbride MJ: Measurement of human nasal ventilation using an oxygen cannula as a pitot-tube. Am Rev Resp Dis 1982; 126: 434–8
19.
Hosselet J-J, Norman RG, Ayappa I, Rapoport DM: Detection of flow limitation with a nasal cannula/pressure transducer system. Am J Resp Crit Care Med 1998; 157: 1461–7
20.
A Manual of Standardized Terminology, Techniques, and Scoring Systems for Sleep Stages of Human Subjects. Edited by Rechtschaffen A, Kales A. Los Angeles, Brain Information Service/Brain Research Institute, 1968
21.
Cheshire K, Engleman HM, Deary IJ, Shapiro C, Douglas NJ: Factors impairing daytime performance in patients with sleep apnoea/hypopnoea syndrome. Arch Intern Med 1992; 152: 538–41
22.
Norman RG, Ahmed MM, Walsleben JA, Rapoport DM: Detection of respiratory events during NPSG: Nasal cannula/pressure sensor versus thermistor. Sleep 1997; 20: 1175–81
23.
Rahman MQ, Kingshott RN, Wraith P, Adams WH, Drummond GB: Association of airway obstruction, sleep, and phasic abdominal muscle activity after upper abdominal surgery. Br J Anaesth 2001; 87: 198–203
24.
Basner RC, Onal E, Carley DW, Stepanski EJ, Lopata M: Effect of induced transient arousal on obstructive apnea duration. J Appl Physiol 1995; 78: 1469–76
25.
Rees K, Spence DPS, Earis JE, Calverley PMA: Arousal responses from apnoeic events during NREM sleep. Am J Resp Crit Care Med 1995; 152: 1016–21
26.
McNicholas WT: Arousal in the sleep apnoea syndrome: A mixed blessing? Eur Respir J 1998; 12: 1239–41
27.
Sleep Disorders Atlas Task Force of the American Sleep Disorders Association: EEG arousals: Scoring rules and examples: A preliminary report from the Sleep Disorders Atlas Task Force of the American Sleep Disorders Association. Sleep 2003; 15: 174–84
Sleep Disorders Atlas Task Force of the American Sleep Disorders Association:
28.
Drinnan MJ, Murray A, Griffiths CJ, Gibson GJ: Interobserver variability in recognizing arousal in respiratory sleep disorders. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 1998; 158: 358–62
29.
Smurra MV, Dury M, Aubert G, Rodenstein DO, Liistro G: Sleep fragmentation: Comparison of two definitions of short arousals during sleep in OSAS patients. Eur Respir J 2001; 17: 723–7
30.
Cracowski C, Pepin JL, Wuyam B, Levy P: Characterization of obstructive nonapneic respiratory events in moderate sleep apnea syndrome. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 2001; 164: 944–8
31.
Pack AI, Cole MF, Goldzmidt A, Ogilvie MD, Gottschalk A: Correlation between oscillations in ventilation and frequency content of the electroencephalogram. J Appl Physiol 1992; 72: 985–92
32.
Khoo MCK, Shin JJW, Asyali MH, Kim T-S, Berry RB: Ventilatory dynamics of transient arousal in patients with obstructive sleep apnea. Respir Physiol 1998; 112: 291–303
33.
White DP: Pathophysiology of sleep apnoea. Thorax 1995; 50: 797–804
34.
Woch G, Davies RO, Pack AI, Kubin L: Behaviour of raphe cells projecting to the dorsomedial medulla during carbachol induced atonia in the cat. J Physiol (Lond) 1996; 490: 745–58
35.
Kubin L, Tojima H, Davies RO, Pack AI: Serotonergic excitatory drive to hypoglossal motoneurons in the decerebrate cat. Neurosci Lett 1992; 139: 243–8
36.
Kubin L, Reignier C, Tojima H, Taguchi O, Pack AI, Davies RO: Changes in serotonin level in the hypoglossal nucleus region during carbachol-induced atonia. Brain Res 1994; 645: 291–302
37.
Cronin A, Keifer JC, Baghdoyan HA, Lydic R: Opioid inhibition of rapid eye movement sleep by a specific mu receptor agonist. Br J Anaesth 1995; 74: 188–92
38.
Lydic R, Keifer JC, Baghdoyan HA, Becker L: Microdialysis of the pontine reticular formation reveals inhibition of acetylcholine release by morphine. A nesthesiology 1993; 79: 1003–12
39.
Mortazavi S, Thompson J, Baghdoyan HA, Lydic R: Fentanyl and morphine, but not remifentanil, inhibit acetylcholine release in pontine regions modulating arousal. A nesthesiology 1999; 90: 1070–7
40.
Keifer JC, Baghdoyan HA, Lydic R: Sleep disruption and increased apneas after pontine microinjection of morphine. A nesthesiology 1992; 77: 973–82
41.
Greenblatt DJ, Schader RI: Anticholinergics. N Engl J Med 1973; 288: 1215–9
42.
Hoijer U, Hedner J, Ejnell H, Grunstein R, Odelberg E, Elam M: Nitrazepam in patients with sleep apnoea: A double-blind placebo-controlled study. Eur Respir J 1994; 7: 2011–5
43.
Loadsman JA, Hillman DR: Anaesthesia and sleep apnoea. Br J Anaesth 2001; 86: 254–66
44.
Aurell J, Elmquist D: Sleep in the surgical intensive care unit: Continuous polygraphic recording of sleep in nine patients receiving postoperative care. BMJ 1985; 290: 1029–32
45.
Freedman NS, Kotzer N, Schwab RJ: Patient perception of sleep quality and etiology of sleep disruption in the intensive care unit. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 1999; 159: 1155–62