Background

Although pain-related activation was localized in multiple brain areas by functional imaging, the temporal profile of its signal has been poorly understood. The authors characterized the temporal evolution of such activation in comparison to that by conventional visual and motor tasks using functional magnetic resonance imaging.

Methods

Five right-handed volunteers underwent whole brain echo-planar imaging on a 3 T magnetic resonance imaging scanner while they received pain stimulus on the right and left forearm and performed visually guided saccade and finger tapping tasks. Pain stimulus on the right and left forearm consisted of four cycles of 15-s stimulus at 47.2-49.0 degrees C, interleaved with 30-s control at 32 degrees C, delivered by a Peltier-type thermode, and visually guided saccade and finger tapping of three cycles of 30-s active and 30-s rest conditions. Voxel-wise t statistical maps were standardized and averaged across subjects. Blood oxygenation level-dependent signal time courses were analyzed at local maxima of representative activation clusters (t > 3.5).

Results

Pain stimulus on the right forearm activated the secondary somatosensory (S2), superior temporal, anterior cingulate, insular, prefrontal cortices, premotor area, and lenticular nucleus. Pain stimulus on the left forearm activated similar but fewer areas at less signal intensity. The S2 activation was dominant on the contralateral hemisphere. Pain-related activation was statistically weaker and showed less consistent signal time courses than visually guided saccade- and finger tapping-related activation. Pain-related signals decayed earlier before the end of stimulus, in contrast to well-sustained signal plateaus induced by visually guided saccade and finger tapping.

Conclusions

The authors speculate that pain-related blood oxygenation level-dependent signals were attenuated by the pain-induced global cerebral blood flow decrease or activation of the descending pain inhibitory systems.

IN the past decade, positron emission tomography studies have yielded substantial insight into the supraspinal substrates of pain experience in humans. Not only the thalamus but also multiple other discrete brain areas are now known to be involved in pain perception, 1including the primary and secondary somatosensory, anterior cingulate, insular, and prefrontal cortices. 2–7These cortical areas receive nociceptive information from the thalamus and are considered to mediate either of sensory-discriminative, affective-motivational, and cognitive-evaluative components of pain experience. 1 

Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has recently become a popular mode of functional imaging in pain research, 8offering higher spatial resolution that permits precise anatomic localization and higher temporal resolution on the order of seconds. Instead of radioisotope tracers, fMRI uses blood oxygenation level–dependent (BOLD) contrast to detect neuronal activation. Briefly, neuronal activation is followed immediately by an increase in regional cerebral blood flow (CBF) to a degree that is greater than the increase in oxygen consumption. This causes an enhancement in the ratio of oxyhemoglobin, which is diamagnetic, to deoxyhemoglobin, which is paramagnetic, in the blood vessels neighboring the activated neurons. This phenomenon is detected as a signal intensity increase in T2*-weighted magnetic resonance images. 9Recent fMRI studies have facilitated further understanding of pain-related brain activity, such as the effects of expectation 10,11and habituation. 12 

Nevertheless, the pain-related brain sites have shown considerable variations among brain imaging studies. 13Although this may partly be a result of differences in experimental conditions, study design, mode and intensity of pain stimulation, habituation, or attentional state of subjects, some other factors unique to pain perception may be involved as confounders, such as pain-induced suppression of the global CBF 14and activation of the descending pain inhibitory mechanisms. 15To solve such uncertainties, more detailed exploration of brain activity changes by pain in well-controlled stimulus conditions are necessary, rather than simple localization by activation mapping.

To this end, the current study was designed not only to localize cerebral activation by a thermal pain stimulus, but also to characterize its signal intensity time courses. A very-high-field MRI scanner was used to enhance sensitivity to pain-related brain activity by BOLD contrasts. 16A Peltier-type thermal stimulator was used that enabled tightly controlled delivery of thermal pain stimulation. 10,12,17In contrast to most earlier reports that used only one-sided (right or left) somatic pain stimulus, we stimulated both sides alternately to confirm the reproducibility and the hemispherical laterality of pain-related activation. We also examined cerebral activation by conventional visual and motor tasks 18,19in the same imaging session. By comparing BOLD signal time courses between the pain stimulus and task paradigms, we tested the hypothesis that pain-related cerebral activation may show weaker, less consistent signal time courses, reflecting possible complication by inhibitory mechanisms.

Subjects

Written informed consent was obtained from five healthy volunteer subjects (aged 22–47 yr; one woman and four men). All were right-handed with no history of neurologic or psychiatric disorders or medications. Subjects had refrained from taking any psychoactive or analgesic medications for a period of 24 h before the study. The protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Pittsburgh.

Experimental Conditions

Each subject was examined with four experimental stimuli or tasks in one imaging session: pain stimulus at the right (PR) and left (PL) forearms; visually guided saccade (VGS); and finger-tapping (FT) tasks. All stimuli and tasks were designed in a “block” paradigm consisting of 15-s (PR, PL) or 30-s (VGS, FT) active phases alternating with 30-s control phases, and given to each subject in a counterbalanced order during the fMRI image acquisition.

Pain Stimulation and Rating of Subjective Sensation

A Peltier-type thermal stimulator (TSA-2001; Medoc, Israel) was used to deliver hot pain stimulus at approximately 46–49°C during the pain experiments. 10,12,17It was equipped with an fMRI-compatible nonferrous thermode (3 × 3 cm2surface) and a 12-m cable, and was controlled with commercial software (COVAS; Medoc) on a laptop personal computer from outside the scanner room. The thermode was fixed on the subject's volar surface of the right or left forearm with a Velcro belt.

Before the imaging session, the thermode temperature for hot pain stimulus was determined individually by averaging five trials for pain threshold with commercial software (WinTSA; Medoc). While the thermode temperature was being increased gradually from 32°C at a ramp rate of 1°C/s, the pain threshold was determined as the lowest thermode temperature required for the subject to report a sensation as “just painful” by pressing a button to stop the stimulus, which lowered the thermode temperature to 32°C at a ramp rate of −10°C/s. The subject was then asked to rate stimulus intensity and unpleasantness separately with an integer between 0 and 10. 7,11In this scale, 0 indicated “no sensation” or “not at all unpleasant,” and 10 indicated “the most intense pain imaginable” or “the most unpleasant feeling imaginable” for the intensity or unpleasantness, respectively. The score of 5 was anchored to “just painful” sensation or “just unpleasant” feeling. All subjects were trained to rate sensation properly before the imaging sessions.

During the pain stimulus experiments, each subject was examined with four cycles of 15-s hot pain stimulus (“on” phase) at the forearm with intervening 30-s control stimulus at 32°C (“off” phase). The stimulus began and ended with an off phase lasting a total of 210 s. At the beginning of the on phase, the thermode temperature was increased from 32°C to the individually calibrated pain threshold at a ramp rate of 10°C/s, and at the end of the on phase it was decreased to 32°C at the same rate. We limited the duration of the on phase to 15 s to reduce head motion-related artifacts caused by subjects’ distress from pain. Immediately after each pain stimulus experiment, the subject was asked to give the scores of pain intensity and unpleasantness verbally through a microphone in the scanner. After the first pain stimulus experiment, the thermode was removed and attached to the other forearm for the second trial.

Visual and Motor Tasks

In VGS and FT, subjects were given visual cues on a projection screen placed above their chest and viewed in an angled mirror fixed to the head coil. The visual cues were presented with in-house stimulus presentation software (CIGAL) 20on a personal computer.

Visually guided saccade consisted of five cycles of 30-s saccade (on phase) with intervening 30-s control fixation (off phase); it began and ended with the off phase, lasting a total of 330 s. The saccade target was a solid white circle, and the fixation target was a white crosshair; both targets subtended 0.75° of visual angle on a black background. The saccade target was presented at 0°, 3°, or 6° of visual angle to the left or right along the horizontal plane and moved unpredictably with a 0.5 probability to the left or right every 0.75 s in a 3°-step from its previous position. During the off phase, the subject gazed at the crosshair at the center of the screen.

Finger tapping consisted of three cycles of 30-s finger tapping (on phase) with intervening 30-s rest condition (off phase); it began and ended with the off phase, lasting a total of 210 s. During the on phase, a series of Arabic numerals (1, 2, 3, and 4) were presented every 1 s repeatedly at the center of the screen, and the subject responded by tapping the second, third, fourth, and fifth fingers against the thumb in both hands, respectively. During the off phase, a crosshair was presented for the subject to do nothing but rest.

Image Acquisition

Magnetic resonance imaging scans were conducted using a whole body 3 T scanner (Signa; General Electric Medical Systems, Milwaukee, WI) with a volume head coil. Subjects lay supine in the MRI scanner with their head immobilized by foam padding and pillow. A visual feedback system, providing the subject with information on the head position, further helped the subject to keep a stationary position. 21Subjects were protected from acoustic noise of the scanner by ear plugs. Functional images were obtained with a T2*-sensitive, single-shot, gradient-echo echo-planar pulse sequence with the following parameters: repetition time = 3,000 ms; echo time = 25 ms; flip angle = 90°; imaging matrix = 64 × 64; field of view = 20 × 20 cm; slice thickness = 3 mm; and slice gap = 1 mm. Fourteen slices were acquired, covering the major part of the cerebrum from the primary sensorimotor cortex to the thalamus in an axial orientation. The imaging trials consisted of 70 consecutive scans (210 s) for PR, PL, and FT, and of 110 scans (330 s) for VGS. After all the functional scans were taken, a structural MRI of the whole brain was acquired using a three-dimensional high-resolution acquisition (fast spoiled gradient-recalled acquisition at steady state) for anatomic reference with the following parameters: repetition time = 25 ms; echo time = 5 ms; flip angle = 40°; imaging matrix = 256 × 192; field of view = 24 × 18 cm; slice thickness = 1.5 mm; no slice gap; and 124 axial slices.

Image Processing and Statistical Analysis

Image processing and statistical analysis of the functional data sets were performed using customized software (IVANA) on a UNIX workstation. IVANA is an in-house package of AVS (Advanced Visual Systems, Waltham, MA) modules developed to generate voxel (volume element)-wise t  statistical maps. 18Before the analysis, low-frequency signal variations, reflecting physiologic fluctuations and inadvertent head motions, were removed by subtracting a smooth low-frequency time course based on a quadratic spline curve fit through four evenly spaced nodes (“detrending”). 20Two data sets of VGS had to be excluded from analysis because of technical difficulties in MRI data acquisition and storage. The data sets from VGS sessions were clipped to 70 volumes (210 s) to equalize the statistical power across all the experiments. The images during the first 6 s of each on and off phase, reflecting hemodynamic transitions of BOLD signal time courses, were excluded from analysis for increased statistical power. 22A voxel was considered activated if a t  test comparing on-phase to off-phase signals resulted in t > 3.5, which corresponded to a chance probability of P < 0.001 without correction for multiple comparisons. For the sake of convenience, such statistical differences are referred to as “activation.” The functional activation maps (t  maps) generated by IVANA were overlaid onto the anatomic reference image and transformed into standardized stereotaxic space (Talairach space) using AFNI software. 23,24To compensate for normal variation in anatomy across subjects, the unthresholded, stereotaxically resampled three-dimensional t  maps were smoothed slightly with a Gaussian filter of root-mean-square radius 0.5 mm. 25The t  maps were merged across all subjects by averaging the t  statistics in each voxel to guard against nonequal MR signal variances among subjects. 25The same threshold of t > 3.5 was used for the resultant merged t  maps. The anatomic brain images were also averaged to produce a “merged” reference image. Activated clusters with a volume less than 0.1 ml, approximately three contiguous voxels, were excluded from analysis.

The voxels of interest for BOLD signal time-course analysis were chosen as voxels showing the highest averaged t  value (local maxima) in some of most intensely activated clusters. For each voxel of interest, all epochs including one off-on-off cycle were averaged across subjects in an event-related manner. The signal changes over the transitions from on to off phases were examined by repeated-measures analysis of variance and a post hoc  Fisher protected least significant difference test for multiple comparisons. The maximum percent BOLD signal changes were averaged across all the epochs and were compared among the different experiments by analysis of variance with a post hoc  Bonferroni test for multiple comparisons.

Stimulus Intensity and Pain Rating Scores

The demographics of subjects and pain rating scores are summarized in table 1. All subjects described the pain stimulus as painful and unpleasant. These pain-rating scores did not differ between PR and PL (paired t  test, P = 0.40 for pain intensity;P = 0.35 for pain unpleasantness). There was no correlation between thermode temperatures and pain intensity scores (r =−0.08, P = 0.91 for PR; r =−0.51, P = 0.43 for PL) or pain unpleasantness scores (r =−0.08, P = 0.91 for PR; r =−0.15, P = 0.83 for PL).

Table 1. Demographics of Subjects and Pain Rating Scores

Average values are mean ± SD.

Table 1. Demographics of Subjects and Pain Rating Scores
Table 1. Demographics of Subjects and Pain Rating Scores

Activation by Pain Stimulation

The most prominent activation by PR was observed bilaterally around the lateral sulcus, i.e. , the inferior parietal lobule and superior temporal gyrus, as shown in table 2and figure 1. The inferior parietal lobule near the lateral sulcus is generally considered the secondary somatosensory cortex (S2) in humans. Other activation clusters appeared in the anterior cingulate (ACC), insular, prefrontal cortices, premotor area, and lenticular nucleus. PL activated similar brain areas but with smaller cluster sizes and less statistical power than those by PR. The ipsilateral S2 and ACC were also activated, as well as the contralateral S2 by PL, but were excluded from analysis because they did not reach the activation volume threshold of 0.1 ml.

Table 2. Cerebral Activation Induced by Painful Thermal Stimulation at the Right and the Left Forearm

t > 3.5 (P < 0.001). Boldface locations were used for blood oxygenation level–dependent signal time course analysis.

Table 2. Cerebral Activation Induced by Painful Thermal Stimulation at the Right and the Left Forearm
Table 2. Cerebral Activation Induced by Painful Thermal Stimulation at the Right and the Left Forearm

Fig. 1. Averaged activation t  maps in axial, sagittal, and coronal planes, generated from functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of pain on the right (PR, top ) or left (PL, bottom ) forearm. Activated voxels are shown by graded colors indicating t  values. The x, y, and z values indicate stereotaxic coordinates of each slice in the Talairach space. R = right side; L = left side; IPL = inferior parietal lobule; ACC = anterior cingulate cortex.

Fig. 1. Averaged activation t  maps in axial, sagittal, and coronal planes, generated from functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of pain on the right (PR, top ) or left (PL, bottom ) forearm. Activated voxels are shown by graded colors indicating t  values. The x, y, and z values indicate stereotaxic coordinates of each slice in the Talairach space. R = right side; L = left side; IPL = inferior parietal lobule; ACC = anterior cingulate cortex.

Close modal

Activation by Visual and Motor Tasks

Visually guided saccade activated the bilateral frontal and supplementary eye fields, posterior parietal, primary and secondary visual cortices (table 3, fig. 2). FT activated the bilateral primary motor and somatosensory cortices (M1/S1), premotor and supplementary motor areas, S2, insular and prefrontal cortices, thalamus, and left lenticular nucleus (table 3, fig. 2). Activation by VGS and FT generally showed higher t  values than pain-related activation.

Table 3. Cerebral Activation Induced by Visually Guided Saccade and Finger-tapping Tasks

t > 3.5 (P < 0.001). Boldface locations were used for blood oxygenation level–dependent signal time course analysis.

Table 3. Cerebral Activation Induced by Visually Guided Saccade and Finger-tapping Tasks
Table 3. Cerebral Activation Induced by Visually Guided Saccade and Finger-tapping Tasks

Fig. 2. Averaged activation t  maps in axial, sagittal, and coronal planes, generated from functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of visually guided saccade (VGS, top ) and finger tapping (FT, bottom ) tasks. Activated voxels are shown by graded colors indicating t  values. The x, y, and z values indicate stereotaxic coordinates of each slice in the Talairach space. R = right side; L = left side; FEF = frontal eye field; SEF = supplementary eye field; IPS = intraparietal sulcus; PCu = precuneus; Cu = cuneus; M1/S1 = primary motor and somatosensory cortices; SMA = supplementary motor area; IPL = inferior parietal lobule.

Fig. 2. Averaged activation t  maps in axial, sagittal, and coronal planes, generated from functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of visually guided saccade (VGS, top ) and finger tapping (FT, bottom ) tasks. Activated voxels are shown by graded colors indicating t  values. The x, y, and z values indicate stereotaxic coordinates of each slice in the Talairach space. R = right side; L = left side; FEF = frontal eye field; SEF = supplementary eye field; IPS = intraparietal sulcus; PCu = precuneus; Cu = cuneus; M1/S1 = primary motor and somatosensory cortices; SMA = supplementary motor area; IPL = inferior parietal lobule.

Close modal

Blood Oxygenation Level–Dependent Signal Time-course Analysis

Average BOLD signal time courses were analyzed at eight voxels of interests in the most robustly activated clusters by each stimulus or task, i.e. , the bilateral S2 and ACC by PR, contralateral S2 by PL, bilateral frontal eye field by VGS, and bilateral S1 by FT (fig. 3). These time courses showed 1–3% of average BOLD signal increases during each stimulus or task with a typical pattern of hemodynamic delay of several seconds both at the beginning and at the end. However, the pain-related signals were rather smaller in amplitude and more irregular than the task-related signals. Although the signal plateaus were well sustained throughout the VGS and FT tasks, pain-related signals tended to decay earlier before the end of stimulus (fig. 4). Although the VGS- and FT-related signals remained above 86% of the maximum until 6 s after the end of task, the pain-related signals decreased to 77% of the maximum before the end of stimulus, down to 56% in 6 s after the stimulus.

Fig. 3. (a ) Averaged blood oxygenation level–dependent (BOLD) signal time courses of representative voxels activated by pain stimulation on the right forearm. Open circles represent the voxel at the Talairach coordinate of (62, −30, 27) in the right secondary somatosensory cortex (S2); filled circles at (−61, −29, 16) in the left S2; and triangles at (1, −1, 43) in the right anterior cingulate cortex. (b ) An averaged BOLD signal time course of a representative voxel activated by pain stimulation on the left forearm. The voxel was localized at (64, −23, 30) in the right S2. (c ) Averaged BOLD signal time courses of representative voxels activated by a visually guided saccade task. Open circles represent the voxel at (44, −12, 56) in the right frontal eye field, and filled circles at (−50, −14, 50) in the left frontal eye field. (d ) Averaged BOLD signal time courses of representative voxels activated by a finger tapping task. Open circles represent the voxel at (42, −26, 55) in the right primary sensorimotor cortex, and filled circles at (−39, −36, 50) in the left primary sensorimotor cortex. Bars represent SEM.

Fig. 3. (a ) Averaged blood oxygenation level–dependent (BOLD) signal time courses of representative voxels activated by pain stimulation on the right forearm. Open circles represent the voxel at the Talairach coordinate of (62, −30, 27) in the right secondary somatosensory cortex (S2); filled circles at (−61, −29, 16) in the left S2; and triangles at (1, −1, 43) in the right anterior cingulate cortex. (b ) An averaged BOLD signal time course of a representative voxel activated by pain stimulation on the left forearm. The voxel was localized at (64, −23, 30) in the right S2. (c ) Averaged BOLD signal time courses of representative voxels activated by a visually guided saccade task. Open circles represent the voxel at (44, −12, 56) in the right frontal eye field, and filled circles at (−50, −14, 50) in the left frontal eye field. (d ) Averaged BOLD signal time courses of representative voxels activated by a finger tapping task. Open circles represent the voxel at (42, −26, 55) in the right primary sensorimotor cortex, and filled circles at (−39, −36, 50) in the left primary sensorimotor cortex. Bars represent SEM.

Close modal

Fig. 4. Decay of blood oxygenation level–dependent signals after the end of stimulus or task. Pain-related signals (filled circles; n = 79) decayed more rapidly than visual and motor task-related signals (open circles; n = 48). Signal intensities were averaged and compared with the maximum (max) during the on phase across all the voxels of interest in all subjects. Bars represent SEM. *P < 0.05; †P < 0.005; ‡P < 0.0001 against the maximum by repeated-measures analysis of variance and a post hoc  Fisher protected least significant difference test for multiple comparisons.

Fig. 4. Decay of blood oxygenation level–dependent signals after the end of stimulus or task. Pain-related signals (filled circles; n = 79) decayed more rapidly than visual and motor task-related signals (open circles; n = 48). Signal intensities were averaged and compared with the maximum (max) during the on phase across all the voxels of interest in all subjects. Bars represent SEM. *P < 0.05; †P < 0.005; ‡P < 0.0001 against the maximum by repeated-measures analysis of variance and a post hoc  Fisher protected least significant difference test for multiple comparisons.

Close modal

The maximum percent BOLD signal changes by pain stimulation in each off-on-off cycle were comparable to those by VGS and FT (fig. 5). In PR, the S2 activation was significantly greater on the contralateral (left) side than on the ipsilateral (right) side. The contralateral S2 activation by PR was significantly greater than that by PL.

Fig. 5. Comparison of maximum percent blood oxygenation level–dependent (BOLD) signal changes among representative activated voxels by pain stimulation on the right (PR) and left (PL) forearm, visually guided saccade (VGS), and finger tapping (FT) tasks. The activation at the contralateral secondary somatosensory cortex (S2) by PR was greater than that at the ipsilateral S2 (*P = 0.0003 by analysis of variance and a post hoc  Bonferroni test for multiple comparisons). The contralateral S2 activation was greater in PR than that in PL (**P < 0.0001). Bars represent SEM. R = right side; L = left side; ACC = anterior cingulate cortex; FEF = frontal eye field; S1 = primary somatosensory cortex. Numbers in parentheses indicate numbers of off-on-off cycles from which maximum BOLD signal changes were sampled.

Fig. 5. Comparison of maximum percent blood oxygenation level–dependent (BOLD) signal changes among representative activated voxels by pain stimulation on the right (PR) and left (PL) forearm, visually guided saccade (VGS), and finger tapping (FT) tasks. The activation at the contralateral secondary somatosensory cortex (S2) by PR was greater than that at the ipsilateral S2 (*P = 0.0003 by analysis of variance and a post hoc  Bonferroni test for multiple comparisons). The contralateral S2 activation was greater in PR than that in PL (**P < 0.0001). Bars represent SEM. R = right side; L = left side; ACC = anterior cingulate cortex; FEF = frontal eye field; S1 = primary somatosensory cortex. Numbers in parentheses indicate numbers of off-on-off cycles from which maximum BOLD signal changes were sampled.

Close modal

The current study showed that the most prominent cerebral activation by thermal pain is found in the bilateral S2, in accordance with earlier fMRI studies using electrical, 26thermal, 12,17,27and laser-evoked 11somatic pain stimulation. Although the t  values of both sides of S2 activation were comparable, BOLD signal time-course analysis clearly showed the contralateral dominance of S2 activation by pain. This is consistent with earlier electrophysiologic findings that showed a higher amplitude of source potential at the contralateral S2 than at the ipsilateral S2 in response to laser-evoked somatic 28and trigeminal pain. 29In humans, S2 is localized in the parietal operculum, lateral and posterior to the face presentation in the primary somatosensory cortex, and anterior and medial to the primary auditory areas. 30,31Receiving afferent fibers from the ventral posterior nucleus of the thalamus and the ipsilateral and contralateral primary sensory cortex, 32,33S2 represents somatotopic organization of the bilateral, predominantly contralateral, body parts, and is involved in the integrative aspect of sensation 32as well as nociception as part of the lateral nociceptive system. 1 

We also observed some pain-related activation clusters in the bilateral temporal opercular region near S2. Although these clusters had the highest intensity on the superior temporal cortex (Brodmann's area 42/22;table 2), they partly overlapped the parietal and frontal lobes on the merged anatomic reference, possibly reflecting activation at S2, insular cortex, and premotor area. Activation of the superior temporal cortex by similar thermal stimuli at 46 and 41°C was also reported by Becerra et al.  12This activation may not be involved in pain perception itself, but may be possibly related to shifting of spatial attention to the pain stimulus. 34Lesion of this area is known to cause spatial neglect in humans and monkeys. 34 

The other activation clusters by pain in the ACC, insular, prefrontal cortices, premotor area, and lenticular nucleus are in accordance with earlier fMRI findings. 10–12,17,27,35,36ACC has nociceptive neurons 37and is considered to mediate affective 38and attentional 39aspects of pain as part of the medial nociceptive system. 1The present ACC activation belongs to the midcingular cognitive region 40that may be involved in attentional orienting reaction 13or motor response to pain, 27rather than to the rostral affective region. 40The insular cortex also receives direct projections from the thalamus and has thermosensory and nociceptive representation. 41Activation of the prefrontal cortex, premotor area, and lenticular nucleus may reflect judging and planning for movement to avoid injury. 7,32 

On the other hand, S1 was not activated by pain, in contrast to its robust activation by FT. Although the role for S1 in pain perception remains controversial among brain imaging studies, S1 is considered to mediate the sensory-discriminative aspect of pain, such as localization and intensity coding, 42as part of the lateral nociceptive system. 1The absence of pain-related S1 activation may be a result of the small size (9 cm2) of the thermode that may have failed to activate sufficient number of cortical nociceptive neurons, which are sparsely distributed, to establish spatial summation. 13In addition, because of the highly tuned somatotopy of S1 and individual differences in sulcal anatomy, intersubject averaging may have obscured possible S1 activation in the activation t  maps. 42 

Visually guided saccade and FT reproduced previously reported oculomotor- and motor-related activation 18,19robustly, with higher t  values than the pain stimulus. This may reflect consistency of BOLD signal changes and well-sustained signal plateau throughout the tasks. Relatively less robust activation by pain may partly be caused by inconsistent signal time course and early signal decay before the end of stimulus, as shown in figures 3 and 4, in support of our hypothesis. An earlier positron emission tomography study demonstrated that pain stimulus by intradermal injection of capsaicin decreased global CBF by 22.8% in normal human subjects, implying that pain stimulus directly activated the sympathetic innervation of cerebral blood vessels. 14Such decrease in global CBF could attenuate or eclipse regional CBF increases by pain and may explain early decay of pain-related BOLD signals when compared with the other nonpainful visual and motor tasks, assuming that changes in global and regional CBF are additive. 43Indeed, hyperventilation-induced hypocapnia, reducing global CBF by cerebrovascular constriction, decreased the BOLD responses to visual stimulus in the visual cortex from 3.97% to 0.77% in normal human subjects. 44The same inhibitory mechanism may have resulted in inconsistent pain-evoked BOLD signals that yielded lower statistical intensity of activation. In contrast, conventional cognitive tasks seem to increase global CBF. 45The visual and motor tasks in the current study also required the subjects’ cognitive efforts and may have sustained or increased global CBF, resulting in well-sustained signal plateau.

The aforementioned speculation is limited by the lack of direct CBF measurement that may reveal interactions between global and regional CBF changes. Indirect nature of BOLD signals does not allow simple translation of negative BOLD contrast to a CBF decrease. It is also unknown whether such fluctuation of global CBF occurs rapidly enough to affect BOLD signals in response to acute thermal pain as well as to capsaicin-evoked pain that persists much longer. 14 

Alternative physiologic factors modifying pain-related BOLD signals may include the effects of acute habituation of pain sensation, diminished attention to a pain stimulus, and descending inhibition of pain.

Behavioral habituation to pain, as was observed with a laser-evoked pain stimulus, 7could parallel attenuation of cerebral activation. We did not measure possible gradual changes in pain sensation over time during each cycle of pain stimulus. Two earlier fMRI studies, however, support our assumption that such behavioral habituation was not likely in the current study. Becerra et al.  12measured pain-rating scores for each of four 46°C-stimuli in a similar block-design paradigm with the same thermal stimulator as ours and found no habituation in pain sensation, despite a progressive decrease in BOLD signal amplitudes toward the end of imaging sessions, which may imply attenuation of neuronal or hemodynamic responses. In contrast, they showed that nonpainful stimulus at 41°C resulted in a decreasing trend of pain sensation. Apkarian et al.  46observed continuous pain-rating responses of subjects using a “potentiometer” while six cycles of 35-s hot pain stimulus were applied and observed an increasing trend of pain sensation during each cycle, and even a gradual increase of the maximum over cycles (sensitization to pain) after the second cycle. The intensity of pain stimulus in our study, averaging 47.7°C, was high enough to avoid acute behavioral habituation, the pain-rating scores being consistently in the “painful” range of 6–9 out of 10 (table 1), although we calibrated the thermode temperature at each subject's pain threshold. The 15-s stimulus may have been more painful than the brief calibration stimulus of the same temperature that was terminated immediately on button pressing. In addition, slow conduction of nociception via  C fibers, and delay between the subjects’ judgment and button-pressing action, may have resulted in calibration at higher temperatures than actual pain thresholds.

Attention directed toward a pain stimulus was shown to enhance pain-related cerebral activation. 39Because the subjects were asked to rate pain sensation for each stimulus session, the level of attention toward the pain stimulus was most likely sustained in the current study. Possible gradual enhancement of pain sensation over time 46would also exclude the factor of diminished attention.

Ascending nociceptive information activates supraspinal analgesic mechanisms mediated by the descending inhibitory systems, which modulate the transmission of nociception at the spinal and medullary dorsal horn via  descending fibers through the midbrain periaqueductal gray. 15Such top-down-regulation may possibly attenuate pain-related cerebral activation indirectly by suppressing nociceptive transmission at the dorsal horn level, or directly by unknown interactions among the cortical and subcortical structures that may be involved in cognitive and motivational processes. 15Although we did not observe the behavioral attenuation of pain sensation, activation of top-down pain control systems may have affected the BOLD signals at the neuronal and hemodynamic levels.

Despite the equal perceived intensity and unpleasantness by PR and PL, PL induced less brain activation than PR, both in the number of activated clusters and in the maximum BOLD signal change at the contralateral S2. Such weaker responses by PL than by PR may imply hemispherical asymmetry in somatosensory representation and support the findings by magnetic source imaging that the representation of the dominant hand was larger than that of the contralateral one in the corresponding hemispheres in right-handers, but not in left-handers. 47 

The current study has the following limitations. First, the conventional block design involved repetition of identical stimuli with fixed duration and intervals, which may have confounded the results with the effects of expectation by subjects. 10This problem could be avoided by using event-related study designs with randomized stimuli. 11,48,49Second, pain stimulus lasting more than 15 s might have been more appropriate to detect early signal decay. However, our preliminary studies with pain stimulus lasting 30 s or longer resulted in significant head motion from distress, which prevented proper fMRI data analysis. Therefore, we chose shorter, 15-s duration of pain stimulus, which eventually resulted in fMRI data that could be analyzed. More sophisticated techniques to correct head-motion artifacts, rather than time-course detrending, might permit longer duration of pain stimulus. Lastly, the functional scans of the current study did not include the lowest part of the cerebrum, such as the hippocampus and amygdala, and the cerebellum. These parts have been studied in relation to pain perception, but not in detail. 7,10To better characterize activation of each brain site involved in pain perception, further fMRI studies with fewer focused brain slice selection and rapid event-related designs will be needed in the future. Such studies will not only facilitate physiologic insight into cerebral pain processing, but will also provide novel methods to evaluate subjective pain experience and the effects of therapeutic interventions or drugs in an objective manner, as was done using positron emission tomography. 50–52 

In summary, we have demonstrated that the bilateral S2 was most robustly activated by somatic thermal pain in BOLD-contrast fMRI. Such activation was dominant on the contralateral hemisphere and more robust during the right-sided stimulation in right-handed subjects. The pain-related activation showed lower statistical intensity and early signal decay in contrast to robust and well-sustained activation by visual and motor tasks. Such attenuation of pain-related BOLD signals may result from the pain-induced global CBF decrease or activation of the descending pain inhibitory systems.

The authors thank Denise Davis, B.S., R.T. (Research Instructor, Department of Anesthesiology and Critical Care Medicine, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania), for technical assistance during magnetic resonance imaging scanning; James Voyvodic, Ph.D. (Assistant Research Professor, Department of Radiology, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina), for providing paradigm presentation and data processing software; and Stuart W. G. Derbyshire, Ph.D. (Research Assistant Professor, Department of Anesthesiology and Critical Care Medicine, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania), for helpful comments on the manuscript.

1.
Treede RD, Kenshalo DR, Gracely RH, Jones AK: The cortical representation of pain. Pain 1999; 79: 105–11
2.
Jones AK, Brown WD, Friston KJ, Qi LY, Frackowiak RS: Cortical and subcortical localization of response to pain in man using positron emission tomography. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 1991; 244: 39–44
3.
Talbot JD, Marrett S, Evans AC, Meyer E, Bushnell MC, Duncan GH: Multiple representations of pain in human cerebral cortex. Science 1991; 251: 1355–8
4.
Casey KL, Minoshima S, Berger KL, Koeppe RA, Morrow TJ, Frey KA: Positron emission tomographic analysis of cerebral structures activated specifically by repetitive noxious heat stimuli. J Neurophysiol 1994; 71: 802–7
5.
Coghill RC, Talbot JD, Evans AC, Meyer E, Gjedde A, Bushnell MC, Duncan GH: Distributed processing of pain and vibration by the human brain. J Neurosci 1994; 14: 4095–108
6.
Craig AD, Reiman EM, Evans A, Bushnell MC: Functional imaging of an illusion of pain. Nature 1996; 384: 258–60
7.
Derbyshire SW, Jones AK, Gyulai F, Clark S, Townsend D, Firestone LL: Pain processing during three levels of noxious stimulation produces differential patterns of central activity. Pain 1997; 73: 431–45
8.
Davis KD, Wood ML, Crawley AP, Mikulis DJ: fMRI of human somatosensory and cingulate cortex during painful electrical nerve stimulation. Neuroreport 1995; 7: 321–5
9.
Ogawa S, Menon RS, Kim SG, Ugurbil K: On the characteristics of functional magnetic resonance imaging of the brain. Annu Rev Biophys Biomol Struct 1998; 27: 447–74
10.
Ploghaus A, Tracey I, Gati JS, Clare S, Menon RS, Matthews PM, Rawlins JN: Dissociating pain from its anticipation in the human brain. Science 1999; 284: 1979–81
11.
Sawamoto N, Honda M, Okada T, Hanakawa T, Kanda M, Fukuyama H, Konishi J, Shibasaki H: Expectation of pain enhances responses to nonpainful somatosensory stimulation in the anterior cingulate cortex and parietal operculum/posterior insula: An event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci 2000; 20: 7438–45
12.
Becerra LR, Breiter HC, Stojanovic M, Fishman S, Edwards A, Comite AR, Gonzalez RG, Borsook D: Human brain activation under controlled thermal stimulation and habituation to noxious heat: An fMRI study. Magn Reson Med 1999; 41: 1044–57
13.
Peyron R, Laurent B, Garcia-Larrea L: Functional imaging of brain responses to pain: A review and meta-analysis (2000). Neurophysiol Clin 2000; 30: 263–88
14.
Coghill RC, Sang CN, Berman KF, Bennett GJ, Iadarola MJ: Global cerebral blood flow decreases during pain. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 1998; 18: 141–7
15.
Bonica JJ, Yaksh TL, Liebeskind JC, Pechnick RN, Depaulis A: Biochemistry and modulation of nociception and pain, The Management of Pain, 2nd edition. Edited by Bonica JJ. Philadelphia, Lea & Febiger, 1990, pp 95–121
16.
Thulborn KR, Chang SY, Shen GX, Voyvodic JT: High-resolution echo-planar fMRI of human visual cortex at 3.0 tesla. NMR Biomed 1997; 10: 183–90
17.
Davis KD, Kwan CL, Crawley AP, Mikulis DJ: Functional MRI study of thalamic and cortical activations evoked by cutaneous heat, cold, and tactile stimuli. J Neurophysiol 1998; 80: 1533–46
18.
Luna B, Thulborn KR, Strojwas MH, McCurtain BJ, Berman RA, Genovese CR, Sweeney JA: Dorsal cortical regions subserving visually guided saccades in humans: An fMRI study. Cereb Cortex 1998; 8: 40–7
19.
Shibasaki H, Sadato N, Lyshkow H, Yonekura Y, Honda M, Nagamine T, Suwazono S, Magata Y, Ikeda A, Miyazaki M: Both primary motor cortex and supplementary motor area play an important role in complex finger movement. Brain 1993; 116: 1387–98
20.
Voyvodic JT: Real-time fMRI paradigm control, physiology, and behavior combined with near real-time statistical analysis. Neuroimage 1999; 10: 91–106
21.
Thulborn KR: Visual feedback to stabilize head position for fMRI. Magn Reson Med 1999; 41: 1039–43
22.
Just MA, Carpenter PA, Keller TA, Eddy WF, Thulborn KR: Brain activation modulated by sentence comprehension. Science 1996; 274: 114–6
23.
Cox RW: AFNI: Software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput Biomed Res 1996; 29: 162–73
24.
Talairach J, Tournoux P: Co-planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain. New York, Thieme Medical Publishers, 1988
25.
Binder JR, Frost JA, Hammeke TA, Cox RW, Rao SM, Prieto T: Human brain language areas identified by functional magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci 1997; 17: 353–62
26.
Oshiro Y, Fuijita N, Tanaka H, Hirabuki N, Nakamura H, Yoshiya I: Functional mapping of pain-related activation with echo-planar MRI: Significance of the SII-insular region. Neuroreport 1998; 9: 2285–9
27.
Gelnar PA, Krauss BR, Sheehe PR, Szeverenyi NM, Apkarian AV: A comparative fMRI study of cortical representations for thermal painful, vibrotactile, and motor performance tasks. Neuroimage 1999; 10: 460–82
28.
Tarkka IM, Treede RD: Equivalent electrical source analysis of pain-related somatosensory evoked potentials elicited by a CO2 laser. J Clin Neurophysiol 1993; 10: 513–9
29.
Bromm B, Chen AC: Brain electrical source analysis of laser evoked potentials in response to painful trigeminal nerve stimulation. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1995; 95: 14–26
30.
Maldjian JA, Gottschalk A, Patel RS, Pincus D, Detre JA, Alsop DC: Mapping of secondary somatosensory cortex activation induced by vibrational stimulation: An fMRI study. Brain Res 1999; 824: 291–5
31.
Treede RD, Apkarian AV, Bromm B, Greenspan JD, Lenz FA: Cortical representation of pain: Functional characterization of nociceptive areas near the lateral sulcus. Pain 2000; 87: 113–9
32.
Barr ML, Kiernan JA: The Human Nervous System: An Anatomical Viewpoint, 6th edition. Philadelphia, Lippincott, 1993, pp 238–50
33.
Carpenter MB: Core Text of Neuroanatomy, 3rd edition. Baltimore, Williams & Wilkins, 1985, pp 348–90
34.
Macaluso E, Frith CD, Driver J: Multimodal mechanisms of attention related to rates of spatial shifting in vision and touch. Exp Brain Res 2001; 137: 445–54
35.
Davis KD, Taylor SJ, Crawley AP, Wood ML, Mikulis DJ: Functional MRI of pain- and attention-related activations in the human cingulate cortex. J Neurophysiol 1997; 77: 3370–80
36.
Porro CA, Cettolo V, Francescato MP, Baraldi P: Temporal and intensity coding of pain in human cortex. J Neurophysiol 1998; 80: 3312–20
37.
Hutchison WD, Davis KD, Lozano AM, Tasker RR, Dostrovsky JO: Pain-related neurons in the human cingulate cortex. Nat Neurosci 1999; 2: 403–5
38.
Rainville P, Duncan GH, Price DD, Carrier B, Bushnell MC: Pain affect encoded in human anterior cingulate but not somatosensory cortex. Science 1997; 277: 968–71
39.
Peyron R, Garcia-Larrea L, Gregoire MC, Costes N, Convers P, Lavenne F, Mauguiere F, Michel D, Laurent B: Haemodynamic brain responses to acute pain in humans: Sensory and attentional networks. Brain 1999; 122: 1765–79
40.
Devinsky O, Morrell MJ, Vogt BA: Contributions of anterior cingulate cortex to behaviour. Brain 1995; 118: 279–306
41.
Craig AD, Chen K, Bandy D, Reiman EM: Thermosensory activation of insular cortex. Nat Neurosci 2000; 3: 184–90
42.
Bushnell MC, Duncan GH, Hofbauer RK, Ha B, Chen JI, Carrier B: Pain perception: Is there a role for primary somatosensory cortex? Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1999; 96: 7705–9
43.
Ramsay SC, Murphy K, Shea SA, Friston KJ, Lammertsma AA, Clark JC, Adams L, Guz A, Frackowiak RS: Changes in global cerebral blood flow in humans: Effect on regional cerebral blood flow during a neural activation task. J Physiol (Lond) 1993; 471: 521–34
44.
Weckesser M, Posse S, Olthoff U, Kemna L, Dager S, Muller-Gartner HW: Functional imaging of the visual cortex with bold-contrast MRI: Hyperventilation decreases signal response. Magn Reson Med 1999; 41: 213–6
45.
Madsen PL, Hasselbalch SG, Hagemann LP, Olsen KS, Bulow J, Holm S, Wildschiodtz G, Paulson OB, Lassen NA: Persistent resetting of the cerebral oxygen/glucose uptake ratio by brain activation: Evidence obtained with the Kety-Schmidt technique. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 1995; 15: 485–91
46.
Apkarian AV, Darbar A, Krauss BR, Gelnar PA, Szeverenyi NM: Differentiating cortical areas related to pain perception from stimulus identification: Temporal analysis of fMRI activity. J Neurophysiol 1999; 81: 2956–63
47.
Soros P, Knecht S, Imai T, Gurtler S, Lutkenhoner B, Ringelstein EB, Henningsen H: Cortical asymmetries of the human somatosensory hand representation in right- and left-handers. Neurosci Lett 1999; 271: 89–92
48.
Buckner RL, Bandettini PA, O'Craven KM, Savoy RL, Petersen SE, Raichle ME, Rosen BR: Detection of cortical activation during averaged single trials of a cognitive task using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1996; 93: 14878–83
49.
Davis KD, Kwan CL, Crawley AP, Mikulis DJ: Event-related fMRI of pain: entering a new era in imaging pain. Neuroreport 1998; 9: 3019–23
50.
Firestone LL, Gyulai F, Mintun M, Adler LJ, Urso K, Winter PM: Human brain activity response to fentanyl imaged by positron emission tomography. Anesth Analg 1996; 82: 1247–51
51.
Gyulai FE, Firestone LL, Mintun MA, Winter PM: In vivo imaging of nitrous oxide-induced changes in cerebral activation during noxious heat stimuli. A nesthesiology 1997; 86: 538–48
52.
Kurata J, Gyulai FE, Firestone LL: Use of positron emission tomography to measure brain activity responses to fentanyl analgesia. Curr Rev Pain 1999; 3: 359–66