AMONG the earliest systematic observations of the physiologic effects of anesthetic agents was John Snow’s description, in 1847, of the various stages of ether anesthesia. Although the focus has evolved somewhat, our interest in measures of the depth of anesthesia has persisted. Although the concern was initially largely one of avoiding the hazards of overdose, we have added a greater interest in the prevention of “underdosage.” There is considerable interest in preventing potentially hazardous hemodynamic and movement responses and in preventing recall. The latter concern applies most particularly to the patient who has received neuromuscular blocking agents. The contemporary literature also indicates an interest in using depth of anesthesia monitors as a means of controlling cost. The hope is that precise titration of anesthetic agents, as guided by a monitor of anesthetic depth, can serve to avoid wastage of expensive anesthetics and expedite postanesthesia care unit or hospital discharge, or both.

There have been several thoughtful discussions of the monitoring of depth of anesthesia 1–3and of the problem of awareness. 4,Table 1lists many of the techniques or devices that have been proposed or tested as methods for determining depth of anesthesia. A thorough discussion by Heier and Steen 1published in 1996 reviews the status of all but the most recent of those techniques. Briefly, the review leads to the conclusion that, although several techniques allow one to identify statistically significant differences in depth of anesthesia among defined anesthetic conditions for populations of patients, none has the sensitivity and specificity to allow the clinician to draw certain conclusions about depth of anesthesia in the individual patients for whom he or she treats. The then-available (1996) devices served as trend monitors of varying reliability but did not permit conclusive statements about depth of anesthesia in individual patients.

Table 1. Techniques that Have Been Used in the Assessment of Depth of Anesthesia

EEG = electroenchepalography.

Table 1. Techniques that Have Been Used in the Assessment of Depth of Anesthesia
Table 1. Techniques that Have Been Used in the Assessment of Depth of Anesthesia

The purpose of this review is to summarize the developments that postdate the articles cited previously. 1That progress has involved principally two depth-of-anesthesia monitoring methods: the Bispectral Index, known by the trademarked acronym BIS (Aspect Medical Systems Inc., Newton, MA); and the middle latency auditory evoked response (MLAER). The BIS is an empirically derived index that is dependent on a measure of the “coherence” among components of electroencephalography. 5The MLAER uses measurements of the amplitude and latency of the early cortical components of the auditory evoked response. This discussion will focus on developments related to those two methods. In addition, because of the interest on the part of the media, patients, practitioners, and investigators regarding the topic of awareness during anesthesia, the issue to which this review gives greatest attention is: Can the available monitors be used to prevent the occurrence of awareness during anesthesia?

I preface this review by highlighting two difficulties that pervade the literature about depth of anesthesia. The first is the heterogeneity of the end points that have been used in the evaluation of the various monitors. Frequently used end points include hemodynamic responses (heart rate, blood pressure) to noxious stimulus, movement in response to stimulus, response to command, and recall. Recall is further subdivided into explicit (conscious) and implicit (subconscious) memory. These various end points do not appear to be part of a continuum and can occur independently of one another. A monitor that has some effectiveness with respect to predicting one end point may not predict others. With respect to movement, the explanation may be that this response can occur as a spinal level reflex. Thornton and Sharpe 6suggest that this may explain the relatively poor correlation between cerebrally derived parameters and movement response. The second difficulty is that of a lack of precision with respect to nomenclature. In using the term “consciousness,” investigators and, as a result, the public, sometimes do not distinguish between the ability to respond to command and the ability to form consolidated memory with subsequent recall of intraanesthetic events. Some investigations have equated the ability to respond to command and the ability to remember. The ability to respond to command (which some refer to as “conscious perception”) does not imply the ability to remember; however, with sustained responsiveness, recall becomes increasingly likely. 7The word “awareness” has also been used with variability in meaning. In this review, awareness  refers to situations in which either implicit or explicit recall of intraanesthetic events occurs.

For a monitor of depth of anesthesia to be valuable to the clinician, two conditions should be met. First, not only must the average values yielded by the device in two distinct states (e.g. , hemodynamically responsive vs.  nonresponsive; aware vs.  oblivious) be statistically different, but also the range of values seen in those two states should not overlap. That is, in the ideal, there should be 100% sensitivity and specificity. At a minimum, if what clinicians seek is a specific numeric threshold that can be interpreted to mean “not aware,” it is essential that there be very high reliability in detecting the event of interest, i.e. , essentially 100% sensitivity (no false-negatives). A monitor used in that manner to detect an event that occurs with a very low incidence might cause more events than it prevents if its sensitivity to the event of interest is not approximately 100%. In addition, a reasonably low rate of false-positives, i.e. , a high specificity, will also be necessary for the instrument to be practical. As noted previously herein, many of the techniques studied have been limited by insufficient sensitivity or specificity. Second, the critical threshold values that distinguish depth-of-anesthesia states of interest should not be influenced by choice of anesthetic agent or by patient physiology, including coincident disease states and long-term use of medications. That is, critical thresholds should be constant (or at least should vary very predictably) from patient to patient and anesthetic to anesthetic. What follows leads to the conclusion (for this reviewer) that, although neither technique completely meets the two preconditions for the identification of probable patient awareness, it is the MLAER that comes closest.

The BIS, for several end points, and for several anesthetic regimens, yields the best combination of sensitivity and specificity of any commercially available depth-of-anesthesia monitoring device. In particular, during propofol-induced hypnosis, it is highly predictive of depth of sedation, as judged by responsiveness of the patient to command and tactile stimulation. 8–11It also is largely unaffected by the electroencephalographic pseudoarousal (activation) phenomenon that occurs with some anesthetics during the initial stages of anesthesia. 9A significant limitation, however, is that BIS thresholds do not appear to be independent of the combinations of anesthetic agents administered. 12–15That is, comparable BIS values achieved with different combinations of agents do not represent the same depth of anesthesia. Mi et al.  13brought the BIS to similar levels by use of propofol infusion with or without fentanyl before tracheal intubation. With intubation, BIS increased similarly in both groups, but there were greater increases in blood pressure and heart rate in the propofol-only patients. Vernon et al.  14correlated preincision BIS with movement or nonmovement in patient groups anesthetized using isoflurane–alfentanil or propofol–alfentanil. The mean BIS of movers and nonmovers was significantly different within anesthetic groups (though the standard deviations overlapped, revealing sensitivity and specificity < 100%). However, BIS values did not distinguish isoflurane–alfentanil nonmovers from propofol–alfentanil movers, i.e. , a BIS value that predicted unresponsiveness in one group predicted movement in the other. They concluded that “BIS values may not be independent of the anesthetic agent used.” Mi et al.  15observed that loss of response to command and loss of lash reflex occurred at higher BIS levels in patients who had received fentanyl in combination with propofol than in patients who had received propofol alone. Practitioners who use this device to titrate to an “adequate” depth of anesthesia must be aware that, for a particular anesthetic regimen, the BIS value that indicates “adequate” anesthesia will be vary among patients and that the range of “adequate” values will vary among anesthetic regimens.

Several evaluations of the BIS have sought to identify threshold values for responsiveness to command or stimulation or for the ability to form memory. The precise relation between the ability to respond to command and the ability to form memory is not certain. It is clear that, in some instances, subjects who respond to command do not have memory of the event. However, it has been observed that patients who could repeatedly respond to command formed explicit memory. 7Accordingly, response to command may be a conservative surrogate for awareness, by identifying those who may soon become capable of formulating recall. Flaishon et al. , 16using the isolated-arm technique, administered induction doses of thiopental or propofol using muscle relaxant and followed the BIS until response to command recovered. Patients were not otherwise stimulated. No patient was responsive at a BIS of less than 58. Glass et al.  10correlated BIS with responsiveness to voice in volunteers who received incremental doses or concentrations of propofol, midazolam, or isoflurane. The lowest BIS at which nonresponsiveness to voice occurred was 40, and 95% of subjects were unconscious at a value of 50. Gajraj et al. , 17in an investigation in which propofol was administered by infusion to patients undergoing joint replacement surgery with use of regional anesthesia, recorded the BIS value immediately after the transition to unresponsiveness to command. The lowest BIS value noted was 40. Lubke et al.  18also found evidence of implicit memory formation in trauma patients anesthetized with isoflurane and fentanyl, with BIS values between 60 and 40.

Collectively, these studies indicate that responsiveness to command or the formation of memory, at least implicit memory, may occur at BIS values 18as low as 40. Does this mean that to ensure that no paralyzed patient will be aroused in response to a noxious stimulus that the BIS must be kept less than 40 in all patients? That would probably result in unnecessarily and perhaps hazardously deep anesthesia in many patients. Furthermore, the peer-reviewed data base is inadequate, both in terms of the number of investigations and of the spectrum of physiologic and pharmacologic circumstances in which studies have been performed to allow identification of critical BIS thresholds. With respect to the spectrum of study conditions, it has been common to exclude subjects with a history of neurologic disease, medication affecting the central nervous system, age younger than 18 or older than 75 yr, and alcohol or drug abuse. Although this may be reasonable for initial investigations, it does not constitute a “real-world” test of the effectiveness of the device being studied. Similarly, many investigations have entailed single-anesthetic-agent or carefully prescribed anesthetic formulas rather than the polypharmacy of contemporary practice. Although investigations have been performed in real-world conditions, they have been limited in number. One such study was performed by Sleigh and Donovan. 19Those investigators observed BIS and spectral-edge frequency (SEF) at the time of various events during induction of and emergence from anesthesia while clinicians blind to monitor output applied “standard clinical practice.” The patients were 26 women, American Society of Anesthesiologists physical status I or II, undergoing “minor surgery” (a restricted corner of the real world). The anesthetics consisted of variable doses of midazolam and fentanyl before a slow induction of anesthesia with use of propofol and maintenance of anesthesia with isoflurane and nitrous oxide. The BIS-related observations are displayed in figure 1, which is reproduced from that report. The figure reveals the same problem of sensitivity and specificity for the BIS (the results were qualitatively similar for the SEF) that has been apparent with other monitors of depth of anesthesia. Although the population mean values for BIS (and SEF) during surgery differed significantly from the mean values at the time of first gag or first response to command, there was a substantial overlap of the ranges. Some values recorded during apparently adequate surgical anesthesia were within the range of values seen in awake patients, revealing again incomplete sensitivity and specificity.

Fig. 1. Box plots of Bispectral Index (BIS) at different stages of anaesthesia. “Drop syringe” refers to the moment, during titration of propofol, at which the patient released a syringe held between thumb and forefinger. Reproduced with permission from Sleigh and Donovan, 19with modifications to the wording on the horizontal axis.

Fig. 1. Box plots of Bispectral Index (BIS) at different stages of anaesthesia. “Drop syringe” refers to the moment, during titration of propofol, at which the patient released a syringe held between thumb and forefinger. Reproduced with permission from Sleigh and Donovan, 19with modifications to the wording on the horizontal axis.

Close modal

The BIS monitor may provide useful trend information in individual patients. However, if the objective is to chose a single BIS threshold that would ensure nonresponsiveness and prevention of awareness in all patients, that number would have to be sufficiently low to result in unnecessarily deep anesthesia in a significant portion of the population. In addition, I am concerned that a dependence on a predetermined numerical threshold, as the primary determinant of the adequacy of depth of anesthesia, will result in occasional patients being inadequately anesthetized. Many of the investigations that have suggested suitable numerical thresholds have been performed using homogenous populations of relatively healthy patients who receive a standardized anesthetic. Although some of these investigations may have suggested substantial specificity and sensitivity for specific BIS output numerical thresholds, it is not confirmed that these thresholds will be as robust in real world heterogeneity of the varying combinations of anesthetic agents used by individual practitioners; nor is it confirmed that intercurrent disease states and, perhaps more importantly, medications will not influence the behavior of the coherence measures on which the BIS algorithm depends. What effect do anticonvulsants (diphenylhydantoin, phenobarbital, carbamazepine, antidepressants of many different classes, sedatives and anxiolytics, and long-term administered analgesics have on the numeric parameter derived by the BIS algorithm? How do the relevant thresholds vary with hypothermia, as may be used in either intracranial aneurysm or cardiac surgery? And, as a final caveat, practitioners must appreciate that variation in electrode montage will also alter the derived BIS values. 20There is much to learn before clinicians should contemplate adopting specific BIS parameters as thresholds that will reliably prevent adverse events, including movement, hemodynamic changes, or awareness and recall.

Signal averaging of the electroencephalogram recorded from a mastoid–vertex electrode montage after repeated click stimulation of the ear yields a highly reproducible sequence of wave forms. These wave forms arise, in sequence, from the brain stem, the auditory radiation, the auditory cortex, and association areas of the cortex (fig. 2). The brain stem auditory evoked response (BAER) is resistant to the effects of anesthetic agents. The wave forms that follow the BAER are increasingly sensitive to anesthetics, with the characteristic pattern of change being increases in latency and decreases in amplitude in response to increasing drug concentrations. The early cortical responses, notably Pa and Nb, vary in a dose-dependent and consistent manner in response to the administration of inhaled and intravenous anesthetics. 6,21However, opioids 22and midazolam 23have less pronounced effects than do the inhaled agents and the various intravenous induction agents. Ketamine has no effect. 24 

Fig. 2. Schematic representation of the auditory evoked response. Reproduced with permission from Bailey and Jones. 21Note that the nomenclature varies. Waves P1 and P2 are identified as Pb and Pc by some authors.

Fig. 2. Schematic representation of the auditory evoked response. Reproduced with permission from Bailey and Jones. 21Note that the nomenclature varies. Waves P1 and P2 are identified as Pb and Pc by some authors.

Close modal

The information regarding the use of MLAER in the detection of awareness was thoroughly reviewed by Thornton and Sharpe 6in 1998. Investigations preceding and following that review have focused on the latencies and amplitudes of Pa waves and Nb waves and on the general morphology of the three-wave Pa-Nb-P1-Nc-P2 complex (fig. 2).

Several studies suggest that the MLAER has substantial potential to be an effective discriminator between the anesthetized and conscious states. 17,19,25–29In a study of cardiac surgery patients during near normothermia in the prebypass period, Schwender et al.  25studied the occurrence of implicit memory during the administration of several different anesthetic agents. They observed that implicit memory occurred only in patients in whom the latency increase in Pa was less than 12 ms. That threshold had a sensitivity of 100% for the detection of patients capable of forming implicit memory and a specificity of 77%, i.e. , 23% of patients with Pa less than 12 ms did not form implicit memory. Thornton et al.  26compared ability to respond to command with the latency of the Nb wave. After morphine premedication, thiopental and a neuromuscular blocking agent were administered to permit tracheal intubation. Anesthesia was maintained with 70% nitrous oxide (N20), which was gradually reduced to 50%. An Nb latency of less than 44.5 ms provided 100% sensitivity for identifying patients capable of responding to command (isolated forearm). Specificity was not calculated. Newton et al.  27studied the effect on the ability of volunteers to form explicit memory during inhalation of sub-minimum alveolar concentrations (MAC) of isoflurane. An Nb latency of 47 ms separated, with 100% sensitivity and specificity, those who could and could not form explicit memory for words presented during inhalation. These investigations indicate that, in at least some circumstances, the MLAER can provide 100% sensitivity, albeit with imperfect specificity, to the occurrence of conscious perception or awareness in anesthetized patients. In addition, three more recent comparisons of MLAER derivatives confirm the high level of sensitivity and specificity in the prediction of consciousness that can be achieved with the MLAER. These studies suggest that the distinction between the anesthetized and awake states is sharper, i.e. , there is less overlap in the ranges of conscious and unconscious values, with MLAER derivatives, than is the case with the BIS. 11,17,19,29 

There is no commercially available device for the intraoperative monitoring of the MLAER for the purpose of depth-of-anesthesia evaluation. In addition, there is ongoing evaluation of which derivative of the MLAER, including the latencies of individual waveforms (usually Nb) and derived indices that incorporate information more information about the overall morphology of the MLAER complex, 28,30,31is optimal. Furthermore, the performance of the MLAER in a broad range of patients and anesthetic conditions remains to be explored. Although there is reason to believe that the MLAER may be useful in identifying situations with a risk of awareness, data suggest that it may be less effective in predicting movement in response to surgical stimulus. 32This needs clarification, as does application of the MLAER during cardiopulmonary bypass. Although MLAER is known to be relatively insensitive to temperature change within the ranges commonly used during cardiopulmonary bypass, 33opioids and benzodiazepines have relatively little impact on the MLAER. It is not known whether MLAER has suitable sensitivity and specificity for depth-of-anesthesia monitoring in cardiac surgery with principal use of these agents. As is the case with BIS, knowledgeable application of the MLAER will need investigation in a broader range of patient types and anesthetic conditions.

There is considerable current interest in the issue of awareness. The concern that, in our patients, unnecessary anxiety about the risk of awareness and unrealistic expectations about the ability of the BIS monitor to prevent the phenomenon have developed has already been discussed in Anesthesiology. 34,35It has also been asserted that careful, prospective study with subsequent peer-reviewed publication will be necessary to establish the effectiveness of any putative awareness-prevention device. 35The peer-reviewed literature does not support the notion that any com-mercially available monitor can serve to prevent awareness, although it indicates that useful trend-monitoring of depth of anesthesia and titration of depth of sedation can be accomplished with the BIS. 10,11Furthermore, even in the event of the development of a device that reliably identifies anesthetic states representing a high risk for awareness, episodes of awareness still may occur. The first reason is that depth of anesthesia at any moment is probably the sum of the effects of the anesthetic agents being administered and the prevailing degree of stimulus-related arousal. Even a monitor that meets the stringent sensitivity–specificity conditions suggested above might “fail,” in the context of light anesthesia with minimal surgical stimulus, in the event of a sudden increase in the intensity of stimulus. The second is that there will continue to be situations in which the clinician is limited by failing hemodynamics from administering the anesthetic agents that are otherwise warranted. It is unrealistic to expect any monitor to be proof-positive against the occurrence of awareness.

1.
Heier T, Steen PA: Assessment of anaesthesia depth. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand 1996; 40 (9): 1087–100
2.
Pomfrett CJ: Heart rate variability, BIS and ‘depth of anaesthesia.’ Br J Anaesth 1999; 82 (5): 659–62
3.
Plourde G: Depth of anaesthesia. Can J Anaesth 1991; 38 (3): 270–4
4.
Heier T, Steen PA: Awareness in anaesthesia: Incidence, consequences and prevention. Acta Anaesthiol Scand 1996; 40 (9): 1073–86
5.
Rampil IJ: A primer for EEG signal processing in anesthesia. A nesthesiology 1998; 89 (4): 980–1002
6.
Thornton C, Sharpe RM: Evoked responses in anaesthesia. Br J Anaesth 1998; 81 (5): 771–81
7.
Dutton RC, Smith WD, Smith NT: Wakeful response to command indicates memory potential during emergence from general anesthesia. J Clin Monit 1995; 11 (1): 35–40
8.
Liu J, Singh H, White PF: Electroencephalographic bispectral index correlates with intraoperative recall and depth of propofol-induced sedation. Anesth Analg 1997; 84 (1): 185–9
9.
Struys M, Versichelen L, Mortier E, Ryckaert D, De Mey JC, De Deyne C, Rolly G: Comparison of spontaneous frontal EMG, EEG power spectrum and bispectral index to monitor propofol drug effect and emergence. Acta Anaesthiol Scand 1998; 42 (6): 628–36
10.
Glass PS, Bloom M, Kearse L, Rosow C, Sebel P, Manberg P: Bispectral analysis measures sedation and memory effects of propofol, midazolam, isoflurane, and alfentanil in healthy volunteers. A nesthesiology 1997; 86 (4): 836–47
11.
Schraag S, Bothner U, Gajraj R, Kenny GNC, Georgieff M: The performance of electroencephalogram bispectral index and auditory evoked potential index to predict loss of consciousness during propofol infusion. Anesth Analg 1999; 89 (5): 1311–1315
12.
Sebel PS, Lang E, Rampil IJ, White PF, Cork R, Jopling M, Smith NT, Glass PS, Manberg P: A multicenter study of bispectral electroencephalogram analysis for monitoring anesthetic effect. Anesth Analg 1997; 84 (4): 891–9
13.
Mi WD, Sakai T, Takahashi S, Matsuki A: Haemodynamic and electroencephalograph responses to intubation during induction with propofol or propofol/fentanyl. Can J Anaesth 1998; 45 (1): 19–22
14.
Vernon JM, Lang E, Sebel PS, Manberg P: Prediction of movement using bispectral electroencephalographic analysis during propofol/alfentanil or isoflurane/alfentanil anesthesia. Anesth Analg 1995; 80 (4): 780–5
15.
Mi WD, Sakai T, Singh H, Kudo T, Kudo M, Matsuki A: Hypnotic endpoints vs. the bispectral index, 95% spectral edge frequency and median frequency during propofol infusion with or without fentanyl. Eur J Anaesthesiol 1999; 16 (1): 47–52
16.
Flaishon R, Windsor A, Sigl J, Sebel PS: Recovery of consciousness after thiopental or propofol. Bispectral index and isolated forearm technique. A nesthesiology 1997; 86 (3): 613–9
17.
Gajraj RJ, Doi M, Mantzaridis H, Kenny GNC: Analysis of the EEG bispectrum, auditory evoked potentials and the EEG power spectrum during repeated transitions from consciousness to unconsciousness. Br J Anaesth 1998; 80 (1): 46–52
18.
Lubke GH, Kerssens C, Phaf H, Sebel PS: Dependence of explicit and implicit memory on hypnotic state in trauma patients. A nesthesiology 1999; 90 (3): 670–80
19.
Sleigh JW, Donovan J: Comparison of bispectral index, 95% spectral edge frequency and approximate entropy of the EEG, with changes in heart rate variability during induction of general anaesthesia. Br J Anaesth 1999; 82 (5): 666–71
20.
Hall JD, Lockwood GG: Bispectral index: Comparison of two montages. Br J Anaesth 1998; 80 (3): 342–4
21.
Bailey AR, Jones JG: Patients’ memories of events during general anaesthesia. Anaesthesia 1997; 52 (5): 460–76
22.
Schwender D, Rimkus T, Haessler R, Klasing S, Pöppel E, Peter K: Effects of increasing doses of alfentanil, fentanyl and morphine on mid-latency auditory evoked potentials. Br J Anaesth 1993; 71 (5): 622–8
23.
Schwender D, Klasing S, Madler C, Pöppel E, Peter K: Effects of benzodiazepines on mid-latency auditory evoked potentials. Can J Anaesth 1993; 40 (12): 1148–54
24.
Schwender D, Klasing S, Madler C, Pöppel E, Peter K: Mid-latency auditory evoked potentials during ketamine anaesthesia in humans. Br J Anaesth 1993; 71 (5): 629–32
25.
Schwender D, Kaiser A, Klasing S, Peter K, Pöppel E: Midlatency auditory evoked potentials and explicit and implicit memory in patients undergoing cardiac surgery. A nesthesiology 1994; 80 (3): 493–501
26.
Thornton C, Barrowcliffe MP, Konieczko KM, Ventham P, Doré CJ, Newton DE, Jones JG: The auditory evoked response as an indicator of awareness. Br J Anaesth 1989; 63 (1): 113–5
27.
Newton DE, Thornton C, Konieczko KM, Jordan C, Webster NR, Luff NP, Frith CD, Doré CJ: Auditory evoked response and awareness: A study in volunteers at sub-MAC concentrations of isoflurane. Br J Anaesth 1992; 69 (2): 122–9
28.
Mantzaridis H, Kenny GN: Auditory evoked potential index: A quantitative measure of changes in auditory evoked potentials during general anaesthesia. Anaesthesia 1997; 52 (11): 1030–6
29.
Gajraj RJ, Doi M, Mantzaridis H, Kenny GNC: Comparison of bispectral EEG analysis and auditory evoked potentials for monitoring depth of anaesthesia during propofol anaesthesia. Br J Anaesth 1999; 82 (5): 672–678
30.
Tooley MA, Greenslade GL, Prys-Roberts C: Concentration-related effects of propofol on the auditory evoked response. Br J Anaesth 1996; 77 (6): 720–6
31.
Dutton RC, Smith WD, Rampil IJ, Chortkoff BS, Eger EI II: Forty-hertz midlatency auditory evoked potential activity predicts wakeful response during desflurane and propofol anesthesia in volunteers. A nesthesiology 1999; 91 (5): 1209–20
32.
Kochs E, Kalkman CJ, Thornton C, Newton D, Bischoff P, Kuppe H, Abke J, Konecny E, Nahm W, Stockmanns G: Middle latency auditory evoked responses and electroencephalographic derived variables do not predict movement to noxious stimulation during 1 minimum alveolar anesthetic concentration isoflurane/nitrous oxide anesthesia. Anesth Analg 1999; 88 (6): 1412–7
33.
Doi M, Gajraj RJ, Mantzaridis H, Kenny GNC: Effects of cardiopulmonary bypass and hypothermia on electroencephalographic variables. Anaesthesia 1997; 52 (11): 1048–55
34.
Katz SM: The media and the BIS monitor (letter). A nesthesiology 1999; 90: 1796
35.
Todd MM: The media and the BIS monitor (reply). A nesthesiology 1999; 90: 1797