Abstract
Neurocognitive investigations suggest that conscious sensory perception depends on recurrent neuronal interactions among sensory, parietal, and frontal cortical regions, which are suppressed by general anesthetics. The purpose of this work was to investigate if local interactions in sensory cortex are also altered by anesthetics. The authors hypothesized that desflurane would reduce recurrent neuronal interactions in cortical layer–specific manner consistent with the anatomical disposition of feedforward and feedback pathways.
Single-unit neuronal activity was measured in freely moving adult male rats (268 units; 10 animals) using microelectrode arrays chronically implanted in primary and secondary visual cortex. Layer-specific directional interactions were estimated by mutual information and transfer entropy of multineuron spike patterns within and between cortical layers three and five. The effect of incrementally increasing and decreasing steady-state concentrations of desflurane (0 to 8% to 0%) was tested for statistically significant quadratic trend across the successive anesthetic states.
Desflurane produced robust, state-dependent reduction (P = 0.001) of neuronal interactions between primary and secondary visual areas and between layers three and five, as indicated by mutual information (37 and 41% decrease at 8% desflurane from wakeful baseline at [mean ± SD] 0.52 ± 0.51 and 0.53 ± 0.51 a.u., respectively) and transfer entropy (77 and 78% decrease at 8% desflurane from wakeful baseline at 1.86 ± 1.56 a.u. and 1.87 ± 1.67 a.u., respectively). In addition, a preferential suppression of feedback between secondary and primary visual cortex was suggested by the reduction of directional index of transfer entropy overall (P = 0.001; 89% decrease at 8% desflurane from 0.11 ± 0.18 a.u. at baseline) and specifically, in layer five (P = 0.001; 108% decrease at 8% desflurane from 0.12 ± 0.19 a.u. at baseline).
Desflurane anesthesia reduces neuronal interactions in visual cortex with a preferential effect on feedback. The findings suggest that neuronal disconnection occurs locally, among hierarchical sensory regions, which may contribute to global functional disconnection underlying anesthetic-induced unconsciousness.
Recurrent neuronal interactions between sensory and association cortices are thought to underlie conscious experience
Anesthetics-induced loss of consciousness is accompanied by a breakdown of functional connectivity between these brain regions
The effects of anesthetics on neuronal connectivity in within local neuronal circuits have not been previously reported
In vivo intracortical electrophysiologic recordings from the visual cortices of male rats reveal that desflurane disrupts cortical layer–specific neuronal interactions in local brain circuits
These observations suggest that neuronal connectivity changes in local cortical circuits may contribute to large-scale cortical functional disintegration as a mechanism of anesthetic-induced unconsciousness
Pharmacologic state manipulation is a rational approach to investigate the neuronal basis of human and animal consciousness. How general anesthetics alter neuronal activity in the central nervous system has been a target of scientific investigations for decades.1–6
Former investigations in humans and primates suggested that conscious perception depends on recurrent neuronal interactions along the hierarchy of early sensory to parietal–frontal cortical regions.7–9 Specifically, sensory stimuli reach awareness only if their feedforward processing along hierarchical regions of the cortex is reciprocated by corresponding top-down messages from higher centers. Consistent with this view, preclinical and clinical studies using various methodologies confirmed that pharmacologically diverse anesthetic agents interfere with corticocortical recurrent activity.5,10–12 Specifically, most studies suggest that anesthetics exert a preferential reduction of anteroposterior or frontoparietal directional connectivity upon the onset of unconsciousness,12–15 notwithstanding a few exceptions possibly due to methodologic differences.16,17
Nevertheless, the necessity of prefrontal cortex for consciousness has also been debated.18,19 Thus, it is also unclear if anesthetic-induced unconsciousness is causally related to the observed disruption of long-range connectivity or to a local disconnection within a posterior site such as the “posterior cortical hot zone,”20 although conscious report of visual stimuli does seem to require the involvement of frontal cortex.9
How do sensory cortical regions participate in the anesthetic disruption of recurrent connectivity? Recent investigations in cortical slice preparations suggest that anesthetics preferentially decrease feedback from higher- to lower-order sensory cortex.5 On the other hand, experiments using electrocorticographic measurements find decreased feedforward versus feedback effects in the visual cortex of monkeys.17 However, this has not been tested by a direct measurement of neuronal interactions in the intact animal.
To determine if directional communication in local circuits is suppressed by anesthesia in vivo, we investigated neuronal interactions between the primary and secondary visual cortex (V1 and V2, respectively) in a chronically instrumented, freely moving rat model. We hypothesized that desflurane would reduce recurrent neuronal interactions especially in the feedback direction. To test our hypothesis, we took advantage of the known layer-specific anatomical segregation of corticocortical feedforward and feedback pathways.21 From the simultaneous extracellular recordings of single-unit activity in different depths of two cortical regions, we then calculated mutual information and transfer entropy (primary outcome variables) as measures of layer-specific neuronal interactions between V1 and V2.
Materials and Methods
Animals and Experimental Procedures
The study was approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee in accordance with the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals of the Governing Board of the National Research Council (National Academy Press, Washington, D.C., 2011).
Ten adult male Sprague–Dawley rats of 260 to 330 g weight (mean age, 9 weeks [Charles River Breeding Labs]) were housed in a reverse light–dark cycle room for 5 to 7 days before surgical implantation, and remained there for the duration of the experiment. Food and water access were ad libitum. The surgical procedures were previously described in detail.22 In this work, a custom-designed electrode array consisting of 32 microwires (wire diameter, 33 µm; electrode spacing, 250 µm; row separation, 375 µm; tip angle, 45° [Tucker-Davis Technologies, USA]) was chronically implanted in the visual cortex of each rat. Neighboring microwires within each array had two alternating lengths in order to record from both supragranular and infragranular cortical layers, targeting layers three and five; from here on, designated as L3 and L5, respectively. The array was positioned so that 16 of the microwires were in primary visual cortex (V1) and 16 were in secondary visual cortex (V2). In V1, the shanks resided in a cortical area representing the lateral superior quadrant of the left visual field. The representations in V2 mirror image those of V1, thus they should correspond to those in V1. As a result, eight electrodes targeted each of the subregions designated as V1L3, V1L5, V2L3, and V2L5. The center of the microwire array was 6 mm posterior and 3 mm lateral from Bregma.23 In a subset of animals, the penetration depth of the microwires was histologically verified. The microwire array and its positioning are illustrated in figure 1A.
Microwire array and data analysis. (A) Location of the microwire array in rat visual cortex. Eight wires target primary visual cortex monocular area (V1M) and eight wires target secondary visual cortex mediolateral area (V2ML). The wires have alternating lengths with wire tips residing approximately in cortical layer three and layer five. Only two rows of wires are illustrated; the second row is shown with dotted lines. Scale above is the distance in mm from Bregma at 3.4 mm lateral position. Drawing is based on Paxinos and Watson atlas.43 (B) Spike raster plot for 20 recorded neurons in one experiments (top) and binned binary representation of spiking (bottom). Red dots indicate at least one spike present in each 10-ms bin, green open circles indicate no spike. Each configuration of coincident red dots across neurons represents a population activity pattern. (C) Pairwise cortical regions (yellow and green areas) for which mutual information and transfer entropy were calculated. (D) Time course of average spike rate from all units within V1 and V2 in one experiment. Spike rate was calculated for 10-s time bins and 10-point Gaussian smoothing was applied. Data from consecutive anesthetic states were concatenated, thus the time axis shows cumulative recording time, not real time. Vertical gray lines demarcate anesthetic conditions with percent inhaled concentration of desflurane indicated. L3, layer three; L5, layer five; LPtA, lateral parietal association area; V1, primary visual cortex; V2, secondary visual cortex.
Microwire array and data analysis. (A) Location of the microwire array in rat visual cortex. Eight wires target primary visual cortex monocular area (V1M) and eight wires target secondary visual cortex mediolateral area (V2ML). The wires have alternating lengths with wire tips residing approximately in cortical layer three and layer five. Only two rows of wires are illustrated; the second row is shown with dotted lines. Scale above is the distance in mm from Bregma at 3.4 mm lateral position. Drawing is based on Paxinos and Watson atlas.43 (B) Spike raster plot for 20 recorded neurons in one experiments (top) and binned binary representation of spiking (bottom). Red dots indicate at least one spike present in each 10-ms bin, green open circles indicate no spike. Each configuration of coincident red dots across neurons represents a population activity pattern. (C) Pairwise cortical regions (yellow and green areas) for which mutual information and transfer entropy were calculated. (D) Time course of average spike rate from all units within V1 and V2 in one experiment. Spike rate was calculated for 10-s time bins and 10-point Gaussian smoothing was applied. Data from consecutive anesthetic states were concatenated, thus the time axis shows cumulative recording time, not real time. Vertical gray lines demarcate anesthetic conditions with percent inhaled concentration of desflurane indicated. L3, layer three; L5, layer five; LPtA, lateral parietal association area; V1, primary visual cortex; V2, secondary visual cortex.
One to eight days after surgery, usually around the middle of the day, the animal was placed in a closed, ventilated anesthesia chamber for continuous recording of extracellular potentials in dark environment. The experimental protocol consisted of consecutively increasing and decreasing desflurane levels at 0, 2, 4, 6, 8, 6 4, 2, 0% inhaled concentration (balance, 30% oxygen) for 30 min at each level. The first 15 min was used for equilibration and the second 15 min for recording. Although it was not tested in this study, previous experiments in the same species revealed loss and return of the righting reflex between 4 and 6% desflurane and absent nociceptive response at 8% desflurane.10,24,25 Anesthetic concentration in the holding chamber was continuously monitored (POET IQ2 monitor; Criticare Systems, Inc., USA). Core body temperature was maintained at 37°C by subfloor radiant heat. Extracellular potentials were recorded with the Cerebus system (Blackrock Microsystems, USA) with 250 to 7500 Hz analog bandpass filter and 30 kHz sampling rate. Animals were euthanized with carbon dioxide inhalation followed by pneumothorax.
Data Analysis and Statistics
Extracellular spikes were isolated into single units using the clustering software SpyKING CIRCUS (Paris, France).26 This efficient, template-based method allows simultaneous clustering of units recorded on all channels. When animals were tested on more than 1 day, the experiment yielding more units was used for further analysis. We classified neurons based on their spike waveform and autocorrelogram. Single units with relatively small half-peak width, short trough-to-peak duration, and high overall autocorrelogram were identified as putative fast-spiking interneurons. Neurons with the opposite waveform properties and either burst or regular spiking pattern were identified as putative excitatory neurons. Overall, 37 of 268 (14%) units were classified as interneurons. Because of the relatively small sample size, all neurons were included in further calculations.
After clustering, a binary representation of the recorded spike trains was obtained by binning the spike times at 10-ms increments. This time bin was used to account for the time delay associated with polysynaptic interaction of neurons between V1 and V2.27 It also conforms to the average dendritic integration time of principal neurons.28 As illustrated in figure 1B, the binned spike train consisted of zeros (no spike present within 10-ms bin) and ones (one or more spike present within 10-ms bin). Each distinct arrangement of zeros and ones in a set of coincident time bins represents the momentary activity pattern of the recorded neuron population.
To quantify neuronal interaction between select cortical regions, we first used mutual information (MI). For example, to quantify the interaction between region x and region y, we first calculated unnormalized mutual information (uMI),
where X and Y are the possible coincident spike patterns in region x and region y, respectively, and H(X) is the Shannon information entropy
calculated from the probability distribution p(X) of coincident binary spike patterns in a region x. H(X, Y) is the joint entropy of X and Y. As calculated, H is a configuration entropy of the coincident spike patterns. It measures the uncertainty, and thereby the potential information content, of neuronal spike patterns of the units sampled in each region over time. Mutual information then measures the symbolic information associated with the interaction of the sampled neuron populations between two regions. These calculations were done independently for each animal in each anesthetic condition.
In order to avoid bias by experiments due to different overall unit count and entropy, all mutual information data were normalized to their mean from the nine consecutive anesthetic conditions j within each experiment:
(Normalizing to the mean was chosen because it does not bias the data at any particular condition, e.g., at awake baseline.) In order to estimate directional interactions, we calculated transfer entropy (TE).29 For two regions x and y, can be written as a difference of conditional Shannon entropies:
where Xt and Xt+1 are the possible coincident spike patterns of region x at time t and t+1, respectively; and Yt and Yt+1 are the same in the other region. is the reduced amount of uncertainty in future of X by knowing the past of Y given past of X. Thus,
measures the statistical influence of region y on region x and
measures the reverse. The two quantities are not necessarily equal.
To compare with mutual information, we calculated the sum of transfer entropies (TEs) in the forward and reverse directions, which also gives a symmetric measure. As with mutual information, we normalized each value to the experiment mean:
where j is an index of the anesthetic condition from induction to emergence.
Finally, to assess the asymmetry of transfer entropy, we calculated the directionality index (DI), defined as the normalized difference of the two bidirectional transfer entropies:
The pairs of cortical regions for which mutual information and transfer entropy were calculated are shown graphically in figure 1C. Regions with fewer than four active units were omitted from the analysis; as a result, 6 of 40 (15%) regions were excluded. Because of the applied normalization across the anesthetic conditions, all reported measures are dimensionless.
Statistical testing of the effect of desflurane on mutual information, transfer entropy and directionality index was performed using two-factor ANOVA (general linear method) with the nine consecutive anesthetic states as fixed level and the rat as random level factor variables followed by testing for statistically significant quadratic trend as a function of state. Because eight tests were performed for each of three outcome variables yielding a total of 24 tests, using Bonferroni correction, significance was accepted at P = 0.002 level. No statistical power calculation was conducted before the study. There were no outliers noted or rejected. Randomization methods and blinding was not used. Calculations were performed using MATLAB version 2018b (MathWorks Inc., USA) and NCSS (NCSS, LLC, USA). All data are reported as mean ± SD unless indicated otherwise.
Results
From all experiments, the relative number of units with a minimum of 1 spike per second–activity per all recording sites was close to 70%. At a few contact sites, more than one unit could be isolated, but this was generally rare; thus, most units were from different recording sites. The number of units found in different subregions of V1 and V2 varied from experiment to experiment but on average, they were similar in the major regions, i.e., V1, V2, L3, and L5. Obviously, the subregions containing fewer recording sites yielded proportionally fewer units. Table 1 summarizes the number of active units in each rat and region. Figure 1D illustrates the time course of average spike rate from all units in V1 and V2 in one experiment as an example.
To quantify neuronal interactions, we first calculated mutual information between V1 and V2 within the L3 and L5 and in those combined, between the L3 and L5 within V1 and V2 and in the two regions combined; as well as between the crossed subregions. The group average results of mutual information are shown in figure 2. With the applied sequence of stepwise increasing and decreasing desflurane concentrations, a U-shape change in mutual information was evident as confirmed by the statistically significant quadratic trend (table 2). The unnormalized mutual information values for V1 to V2 and L3 to L5 were 0.52 ± 0.33 and 0.53 ± 0.31 bits at 0% desflurane, which were reduced to 0.33 ± 0.34 and 0.31 ± 0.34 bits, respectively, at 8% desflurane. The proportion change of mutual information from baseline to 8% desflurane concentration was −37% for V1 to V2 and −41% for L3 to L5.
Effect of desflurane on mutual information between neurons recorded in primary and secondary visual cortex (V1, V2), cortical layers three and five (L3, L5), and their subregions. Anesthetic states 1 to 9 correspond to consecutive desflurane concentrations of 0, 2, 4, 6, 8, 6, 4, 2, 0%, respectively. Mutual information is in relative units, plotted as mean and 95% CI. *Statistically significant quadratic trend with consecutive anesthetic states.
Effect of desflurane on mutual information between neurons recorded in primary and secondary visual cortex (V1, V2), cortical layers three and five (L3, L5), and their subregions. Anesthetic states 1 to 9 correspond to consecutive desflurane concentrations of 0, 2, 4, 6, 8, 6, 4, 2, 0%, respectively. Mutual information is in relative units, plotted as mean and 95% CI. *Statistically significant quadratic trend with consecutive anesthetic states.
In addition, upon recovery at 0% desflurane, mutual information was statistically significantly higher than during baseline (by 66% for V1 to V2 and by 61% for L3 to L5; P = 0.002, two-tailed test). The corresponding unnormalized mutual information values were 0.52 ± 0.51 and 0.53 ± 0.51 bits, respectively. In fact, by occasional observations, recovery was often accompanied by a period of behavioral hyperarousal, later followed by relaxed wakefulness or occasionally spontaneous sleep. There was no statistically significant quadratic trend in mutual information among any of the subregions.
By definition, mutual information is a non-directional measure of the interdependence of symbolic information from two sources. In order to assess the effect of desflurane on directional interactions of neurons, we calculated the alternative information theoretic quantity, transfer entropy, and its directionality index that quantifies the imbalance between feedforward and feedback communication.
First, to compare with mutual information, we examined the sum of transfer entropies in forward and reverse directions. Because the transfer entropy sum is nondirectional, it was expected to behave similarly to mutual information. Figure 3 shows that desflurane statistically significantly decreased transfer entropy sum for all region-pairs suggesting a general decrease in both interregional and interlaminar interactions in visual cortex. Because many of these effects were not seen with mutual information, transfer entropy sum appears to be a more sensitive measure of change than mutual information. The changes from 0 to 8% desflurane were also larger with transfer entropy sum, reaching 77% for V1 to V2 and 78% for L3 to L5, with subregions combined as done for mutual information. The corresponding unnormalized transfer entropy values were 1.86 ± 1.56 and 1.87 ± 1.67 bits at baseline 0% desflurane, and 0.43 ± 0.37 and 0.41 ± 0.37 bits at 8% desflurane.
Robust concentration-dependent effect of desflurane on the sum of transfer entropies calculated between primary and secondary visual cortex (V1, V2), between cortical layers three and five (L3, L5), and between various subregions. Anesthetic states 1 to 9 correspond to consecutive desflurane concentrations of 0, 2, 4, 6, 8, 6, 4, 2, 0%, respectively. Transfer entropy sum is in relative units, plotted as mean and 95% CI. *Statistically significant quadratic trend with consecutive anesthetic states.
Robust concentration-dependent effect of desflurane on the sum of transfer entropies calculated between primary and secondary visual cortex (V1, V2), between cortical layers three and five (L3, L5), and between various subregions. Anesthetic states 1 to 9 correspond to consecutive desflurane concentrations of 0, 2, 4, 6, 8, 6, 4, 2, 0%, respectively. Transfer entropy sum is in relative units, plotted as mean and 95% CI. *Statistically significant quadratic trend with consecutive anesthetic states.
We then examined the effect of desflurane on the directionality index (fig. 4). As with mutual information and transfer entropy sum, we found statistically significant quadratic trend of the directionality index between V1 and V2 and between L3 and L5. The positive sign of the directionality index at 0% desflurane shows that in this state, V2 → V1 and L3 → L5 directional effects were dominant (0.11 ± 0.18 and 0.12 ± 0.20 for V2 → V1 and L3 → L5, respectively), but this dominance was reduced during anesthesia by 89% for V2 → V1 and 97% for L3 → L5 at 8% desflurane to 0.01 ± 0.04 and −0.01 ± 0.05 for V1 → V2 and L3 → L5, respectively. Moreover, the reduction of directionality index in layer 5 (V2L5→V1L5) implied a larger reduction of feedback than feedforward connectivity (108% decrease at 8% desflurane vs. 0% baseline; higher than 100% indicates a reversal in direction), consistent with the anatomical disposition of V2 → V1 feedback between the higher-order and lower-order infragranular regions. The relatively large (97%) reduction in overall L3 → L5 directionality could not be partialled into subregions, although the quadratic trend of the directionality index for V1L3→V1L5 was close to statistical significance. Individual mutual information, transfer entropy sum and directionality index data from each animal for the three most relevant interactions are provided in figure S1 in the Supplemental Digital Content (http://links.lww.com/ALN/C245).
Effect of desflurane on the directionality index. Statistically significant change occurred in layer five secondary visual to layer five primary visual cortex (V2L5–V1L5), implying a reduction of feedback connectivity at higher desflurane concentration. Anesthetic states 1 to 9 correspond to consecutive desflurane concentrations of 0, 2, 4, 6, 8, 6, 4, 2, 0%, respectively. Directionality index is in relative units, plotted as mean and 95% CI. *Statistically significant quadratic trend with consecutive anesthetic states.
Effect of desflurane on the directionality index. Statistically significant change occurred in layer five secondary visual to layer five primary visual cortex (V2L5–V1L5), implying a reduction of feedback connectivity at higher desflurane concentration. Anesthetic states 1 to 9 correspond to consecutive desflurane concentrations of 0, 2, 4, 6, 8, 6, 4, 2, 0%, respectively. Directionality index is in relative units, plotted as mean and 95% CI. *Statistically significant quadratic trend with consecutive anesthetic states.
Discussion
The goal of this study was to determine if a common, clinically used anesthetic, desflurane, would alter neuronal interactions locally, between hierarchical stages of early cortical sensory processing regions in an intact animal in vivo. From the simultaneous recording of many neurons sampled at two depths of primary and secondary visual cortex, we found both overall and layer-specific modifications of interregional neuronal interactions by desflurane. Specifically, changes in both mutual information and transfer entropy revealed robust, concentration-dependent reductions of neuronal interactions between V1 and V2 and between L3 and L5. Transfer entropy suggested a more general suppression of neuronal interactions among all sampled regions of visual cortex. Moreover, the transfer entropy directionality index predicted a greater reduction of V2 - V1 feedback than V1 - V2 feedforward, specifically within L5 during anesthesia.
The latter results are consistent with the anatomical disposition of feedback pathways that originate mainly in higher order infragranular layers and target both lower order supragranular and lower order infragranular layers.5,30 It also agrees with the frequently observed anesthetic reduction of large-scale anteroposterior or frontoparietal directional connectivity.10–15,31,32 Here we find that this is also true locally, within visual cortex. Layer-specific modulations of local neuronal firing responses to visual stimulation by isoflurane were also found in the ferret visual cortex.31 Although the magnitude of the relevant changes of directionality index were relatively modest, one could speculate that a cumulative effect of local feedback reductions along several hierarchical cortical areas could result in a more robust shift in the feedback/feedforward balance in large-scale cortical connectivity.
Two recent studies challenge the finding of preferential feedback modulation by anesthesia. First, Sanders et al. found that the primary effect of propofol on transcranial magnetic stimulation–induced electroencephalogram (EEG) cross-frequency coupling was in the feedforward direction although feedback coupling in the beta frequency band was also reduced.16 Clearly, transcranial magnetic stimulation–induced EEG coupling may be different from resting-state EEG33 or sensory-evoked local field potential10 and local unit correlations.25 Second, Ma et al. reported that bolus injection of propofol in monkeys increased electroencephalographic low-frequency phase β-amplitude coupling in frontoparietal and V2–V1 regions such that the phase of higher-order areas modulated the band power of lower-order areas.17 However, such effects of propofol may not apply to those of volatile anesthetics. Previous studies found reduced γ coherence and transfer entropy,10,34 and reduced wideband phase synchrony31 between the frontal and visual cortex during isoflurane anesthesia. Both corticocortical coherence and frontoparietal symbolic transfer entropy in the high γ band were decreased during sevoflurane anesthesia.32
The fact that transfer entropy sum revealed additional, statistically significant effects of desflurane over those predicted by mutual information suggests that transfer entropy, in general, may be a more sensitive measure of neuronal interactions. Mutual information is a probabilistic measure of shared symbolic information of two signal sources; in our case, the binary configuration of coincident neuronal spikes in 10-ms time bins consistent with the hypothesis of population coding of neuronal information.35 A limitation of mutual information is that it does not necessarily measure direct exchange of information between two neuron populations; it may in part indicate the action of an independent common synchronizing input. Under anesthesia, both synchronized spontaneous oscillations and common synchronizing input can contribute to shared information as measured by mutual information. Moreover, deep levels of anesthesia are associated with burst suppression—a visible alternation of brief high activity bursts and intervening silent periods. The bursts themselves are highly synchronized, which counters the decrease in true neuronal interactions. Neuron bursts develop gradually with increasing anesthetic concentration with the duration of bursts (or up-states) becoming shorter and the silent periods becoming longer.25 Thus, in general, mutual information reveals “functional” but not necessarily direct neuronal interaction. The exact nature of neuronal information is also in question; therefore, the word information in the current context should be understood somewhat metaphorically.
Transfer entropy attempts to circumvent the limitation due to common synchronizing input by estimating the probabilistic causal influence of one signal source on another. Specifically, transfer entropy measures the reduction of uncertainty in future values of Y by knowing the past values of X given the past values of Y. If the spike train Y becomes more synchronized and predictable, such as during slow oscillation or burst suppression, knowing the past of spike train Y does not reduce the uncertainty of X (and vice versa). This leads to a decrease in transfer entropy rather than an increase and may explain the difference in behavior between transfer entropy and mutual information. The validity of transfer entropy as a pure measure of causal influence has been questioned,36 as it may be overly influenced by a decrease in source entropy.37 This could also explain why more transfer entropy sum interactions were reduced in anesthesia. Alternatives such as normalized transfer entropy have been introduced38 but not applied here due to the resulting ambiguity in interpretation. Regardless of these considerations, transfer entropy appears to be a superior estimator of the direct influence of one signal on another compared to mutual information.
In order to evaluate a graded, dose-dependent response, desflurane was administered incrementally from wakefulness to deep anesthesia and back, ending with full emergence. All statistically significant effects of desflurane were reversible upon termination of anesthesia. Moreover, the values of both mutual information and transfer entropy sum after emergence often exceeded those in wakeful baseline. The mechanism of this increase is currently unclear but may reflect persistent postinhibitory rebound of neuronal activity.39 Accordingly, there was no indication of neural inertia, a phenomenon thought to delay the recovery from anesthesia.40 This was probably due to the relatively long time course of our experiments (270 min), which presumably facilitated the equilibration of state at each anesthesia level.41
What may be the role of anesthetic modulation of local modality-specific circuits in the overall disruption of cortical connectivity for the loss of consciousness? It is generally believed that under anesthesia, a generalized failure of bidirectional information flow across the entire cortical mantle would prevent any kind of information reaching the level of awareness, presumably equating this condition with complete loss of consciousness. Because the anesthetic modulation of local interactions is statistically significant and robust,24,42 these may, in effect, produce loss of conscious perception in a specific modality. Moreover, the cumulative effect of local circuit disruptions across higher levels of the cortical hierarchy may be amplified to the degree that coerces the generalized loss of consciousness. Thus, our current observations using anesthesia as a model of unconsciousness line up well with the prevailing view derived from cognitive neuroscience investigations stating that conscious perception depends critically on recurrent communication along the cortical hierarchy.7,8
In addition to the analytical considerations above, technical limitations of the present study include the relatively sparse sampling of neurons in four cortical subregions and the possible individual variation in the V1–V2 region boundaries as defined by the Paxinos atlas. In some experiments, there were an insufficient number of neurons to estimate the entropies in all four regions that resulted in missing data and limited the statistical power. Also, we recorded from only two depths of cortex although specific information from all cortical layers may be desirable. The recording sites were presumed to reside in L3 and L5, but the exact location of the electrode positions could not be determined. In some cases, histological verification of the electrode sites was attempted; however, this was hampered by the presence of 32 microwires and the tissue damage their removal caused. Future work could examine neuronal interactions among more precisely defined cortical layers and subregions using high-density microelectrode arrays both within and beyond the visual system. The rats were tested in dark environment during their active diurnal cycle to minimize the chance for spontaneous sleep. Given the relatively long duration of anesthetic exposure using multiple doses, the amount of data collected did not allow us to segment the baseline and recovery conditions to specific behavioral states, such as exploration, quiet wakefulness, and sleep. Therefore, these states may not be strictly equivalent to normal attentive wakefulness and should be best designated as anesthetic-free. In addition, in order to limit the duration of anesthetic exposure, we did not apply any visual stimuli, thus our results apply to spontaneous ongoing neuronal activity in the visual system. Finally, all experiments were performed on adult male rats due to statistical considerations. Possible age and sex differences in the results should be investigated in the future.
We conclude that desflurane disrupts global and cortical layer–specific interareal neuronal interactions in rat visual cortex with a preferential effect on V2–V1 feedback. The findings suggest that anesthesia produces neuronal connectivity changes in local circuits, which may cumulatively lead to large-scale cortical functional disintegration as a postulated mechanism of anesthetic-induced unconsciousness.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Kathy Zelenock, M.S., Department of Anesthesiology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan, for her assistance in general laboratory operations and manuscript editing.
Research Support
Supported by the National Institute of General Medical Sciences of the National Institutes of Health award No. R01-GM056398 (Bethesda, Maryland; to Dr. Hudetz) and by the Department of Anesthesiology, Center for Consciousness Science, University of Michigan Medical School, Ann Arbor, Michigan. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.
Competing Interests
The authors report no conflicts of interests.