We thank Colin et al. and Schwartz for their interest in our recently published work in Anesthesiology.1  In the referenced manuscript, we fitted slow-wave activity drug dose-response curves to electroencephalographic data acquired during anesthesia in a propofol healthy volunteer study and three patient studies. By applying these techniques to induction and emergence, we presented two distinct, but related, findings.

First, as described by Schwartz, we confirmed that our experimental finding of slow-wave activity saturation2  also occurs during surgical anesthesia. While we entirely agree that exploration of the transition to burst suppression is important, this was not the focus of our article. Slow-wave activity saturation occurs at considerably lower levels of anesthesia than burst suppression (fig. 1G of Warnaby et al.1 )—hence why the presence of burst suppression was considered to be artefactual in the fitting of slow-wave activity–concentration curves. Furthermore, we did not imply that muscle relaxants have no influence on the electroencephalogram, only that they had no influence on the slow-wave activity saturation parameters (i.e., the power and concentration of slow-wave activity saturation, defined by the electroencephalographic dose-response curve fit). Neuromuscular blocking drugs certainly influence the electroencephalogram and, in particular, frequencies above 20 Hz,3  but we are unaware of any evidence that neuromuscular blockade potentiates burst suppression in humans. The prerequisites for inclusion of patient data in our analyses were specified in the Materials and Methods section. Any small discrepancies in N were due to the original data not meeting the criteria for inclusion in this different analysis with different criteria.

Fig. 1.

Simulation of drug concentration-response curves for induction and emergence from anesthesia. The raw processes are indicated by the blue curves, with arrows showing the direction of change (i.e., induction = up arrow, emergence = down arrow). The addition of a equilibration rate constant of 0.25 results in the EC50 of induction and EC50 of emergence being equal (i.e., EC50 induction = EC50 emergence, red curves). The resulting figure-of-eight loops indicate failure to properly describe the different process driving induction and emergence. This example highlights the inadequacy of a single equilibration rate constant to model the biologic situation.

Fig. 1.

Simulation of drug concentration-response curves for induction and emergence from anesthesia. The raw processes are indicated by the blue curves, with arrows showing the direction of change (i.e., induction = up arrow, emergence = down arrow). The addition of a equilibration rate constant of 0.25 results in the EC50 of induction and EC50 of emergence being equal (i.e., EC50 induction = EC50 emergence, red curves). The resulting figure-of-eight loops indicate failure to properly describe the different process driving induction and emergence. This example highlights the inadequacy of a single equilibration rate constant to model the biologic situation.

Close modal

Second, as discussed by Colin et al., we presented evidence of neural inertia in humans. We agree that there is a wide variation across individuals in the time taken for the drug to move from the blood and “activate” its effect site, and this is one of the many weak links in the use of population pharmacokinetic models. However, it is important to remember that the equilibration rate parameter (ke0) is an abstract parameter that depends on the elimination of hysteresis for its estimation, therefore providing potential for circularity in this argument. We strongly argue that this single exponential diffusion term is poor at capturing the complexity of the many processes involved. This single parameter is expected to subsume a multiplicity of biologic processes including: (1) chemical diffusion from blood to brain, (2) molecular binding to various receptor targets, (3) disturbance of neuronal function, (4) impairment of neural network dynamics, and (5) the resultant change in behavior.

As shown by the modeling of our experimental propofol infusion regime in the letter from Colin et al., it is mathematically inevitable that a value of the equilibration rate constant can be chosen so that the EC50 of induction equals the EC50 of emergence. However, based on our findings that were obtained using the commonly accepted population-based equilibration rate constant values, we would argue that the shape of the drug concentration effect curve is a better indicator of neural inertia in an individual than the EC50. As an illustration, we ran a drug concentration–effect simulation with two different processes for induction and emergence (fig. 1, blue curves). The addition of a suitable equilibration rate constant will make the EC50 of induction and the EC50 of emergence equal (fig. 1, red curves). However, we believe it is the failure of the induction and emergence curves to overlay that is a true marker that induction and emergence are not equivalent processes. The EC50 is ultimately a single point on the population-generated curve and, as such, is not particularly meaningful for assessing such a complex phenomenon within an individual.

We entirely agree with Colin et al. that a more precise definition of “neural inertia” is required, as there are many subtleties that need to be further elucidated.4  Originally proposed by Friedman et al.,5  a neural component of the observed hysteresis is inferred through genetic and pharmacologic modification in animals also using behavioral state changes as a read-out. We would like to point out that, even using the disputed average population propofol model parameters, we did not find much evidence of neural inertia for behavioral response. This is similar to the result obtained in the recent associated paper by Kuizenga et al.6 

Importantly, we did find strong hysteresis in our brain-based measurement of electroencephalographic slow-wave activity. In particular, the identification of abrupt slow-wave emergence trajectories in some individuals indicates a higher resistance to brain state changes on emergence, i.e., these individuals have a higher degree of neural inertia. This quite marked interindividual hysteresis is lost unless within-subject analyses are performed as we have done. Furthermore, our categorization of abrupt (rather than graded) emergence trajectories takes into account three factors: the electroencephalographic brain state, behavioral state, and hypnotic drug concentrations. While we acknowledge that the use of population-based propofol effect concentrations is a minor limitation, these abrupt trajectories were also identified in the clinical studies using the more accurate measurement of end-tidal volatile agent concentrations.

We believe our findings provide additional support to Kelz and coworkers’ hypothesis that a neural effect contributes to anesthetic hysteresis, further highlighting that this is not purely a pharmacokinetic issue. Based on our findings, we proposed a more general definition of neural inertia as “a tendency of the central nervous system to resist transitions between conscious and unconscious states” in the article’s Discussion. This allows for inclusion of different brain state transitions, as inferred by electroencephalographic methods, functional brain imaging, and/or behavior. While Colin et al. have tended to focus on pharmacokinetic mechanisms and behavioral read-outs, the shortcomings of which for assessing true neural inertia are elegantly detailed by Proekt and Kelz,4  we suggest our systems-level approach is more suitable and indicates that both disruption of neuronal function and impairment of neural network dynamics play a key role in neural inertia.

Dr. Warnaby and Irene Tracey were listed as inventors on patent applications filed by Oxford University Innovation, the technology transfer company of the University of Oxford (Oxford, United Kingdom; formerly Isis Innovation) on perception loss detection using slow-wave activity saturation. The remaining author declares no competing interests.

1.
Warnaby
CE
,
Sleigh
JW
,
Hight
D
,
Jbabdi
S
,
Tracey
I
:
Investigation of slow-wave activity saturation during surgical anesthesia reveals a signature of neural inertia in humans.
Anesthesiology
2017
;
127
:
645
57
2.
Ni Mhuircheartaigh
R
,
Warnaby
C
,
Rogers
R
,
Jbabdi
S
,
Tracey
I
:
Slow-wave activity saturation and thalamocortical isolation during propofol anesthesia in humans.
Sci Transl Med
2013
;
5
:
208ra148
3.
Whitham
EM
,
Pope
KJ
,
Fitzgibbon
SP
,
Lewis
T
,
Clark
CR
,
Loveless
S
,
Broberg
M
,
Wallace
A
,
DeLosAngeles
D
,
Lillie
P
,
Hardy
A
,
Fronsko
R
,
Pulbrook
A
,
Willoughby
JO
:
Scalp electrical recording during paralysis: Quantitative evidence that EEG frequencies above 20 Hz are contaminated by EMG.
Clin Neurophysiol
2007
;
118
:
1877
88
4.
Proekt
A
,
Kelz
M
:
Schrödinger’s cat: Anaesthetised and not!
Br J Anaesth
2018
;
120
:
424
8
5.
Friedman
EB
,
Sun
Y
,
Moore
JT
,
Hung
HT
,
Meng
QC
,
Perera
P
,
Joiner
WJ
,
Thomas
SA
,
Eckenhoff
RG
,
Sehgal
A
,
Kelz
MB
:
A conserved behavioral state barrier impedes transitions between anesthetic-induced unconsciousness and wakefulness: Evidence for neural inertia.
PLoS One
2010
;
5
:
e11903
6.
Kuizenga
MH
,
Colin
PJ
,
Reyntjens
KMEM
,
Touw
DJ
,
Nalbat
H
,
Knotnerus
FH
,
Vereecke
HEM
,
Struys
MMRF
:
Test of neural inertia in humans during general anaesthesia.
Br J Anaesth
2018
;
120
:
525
36