Background

Postoperative cognitive dysfunction is common, but it remains unclear whether there are long-term adverse cognitive effects of surgery combined with anesthesia. The authors examined the association between exposure to surgery and level of cognitive functioning in a sample of 8,503 middle-aged and elderly twins.

Methods

Results from five cognitive tests were compared in twins exposed to surgery, classified as major, minor, hip and knee replacement, or other, with those of a reference group without surgery using linear regression adjusted for sex and age. Genetic and shared environmental confounding was addressed in intrapair analyses of 87 monozygotic and 124 dizygotic same-sexed twin pairs in whom one had a history of major surgery and the other did not.

Results

Statistically significantly lower composite cognitive score was found in twins with at least one major surgery compared with the reference group (mean difference, −0.27; 95% CI, −0.48 to −0.06), corresponding to one tenth of an SD, that is, a negligible effect size. In the intrapair analysis, the surgery-exposed co-twin had the lower cognitive score in 49% (95% CI, 42 to 56%) of the pairs. None of the other groups differed from the reference group except the knee and hip replacement group that tended to have higher cognitive scores (mean difference, 0.35; 95% CI, −0.18 to 0.87).

Conclusions

A history of major surgery was associated with a negligibly lower level of cognitive functioning. The supplementary analyses suggest that preoperative cognitive functioning and underlying diseases were more important for cognitive functioning in mid- and late life than surgery and anesthesia.

In a study of 8,503 twins, a negligible but statistically significant decrease in a sensitive composite cognitive score was present in twins with at least one major surgery, but there was no difference by intrapair analysis. There was no clinically significant association of major surgery and anesthesia with long-term cognitive dysfunction, suggesting that factors other than surgery and anesthesia are more important.

What We Already Know about This Topic
  • Postoperative cognitive dysfunction is common in the elderly, but the contributions of surgery, anesthesia, preexisting conditions, and other factors are unclear

  • A large Danish registry of middle-aged and elderly twins provides a powerful approach to detect subtle effects of surgery and anesthesia on cognitive functioning by minimizing environmental and genetic confounding

What This Article Tells Us That Is New
  • In a study of 8,503 twins, a negligible but statistically significant decrease in a sensitive composite cognitive score was present in twins with at least one major surgery, but there was no difference by intrapair analysis

  • There was no clinically significant association of major surgery and anesthesia with long-term cognitive dysfunction, suggesting that factors other than surgery and anesthesia are more important

COGNITIVE impairment is a well-known complication after surgery with delirium and postoperative cognitive dysfunction (POCD) as the most common disorders.1  Early (7 days) POCD occurs in adult surgical patients of all ages, but elderly patients are at higher risk, and at 3 months after surgery, cognitive impairment is still detectable in patients older than 60 yr.2–5  POCD may be present for longer than 6 months in some patients6  with potential long-term implications for the patient including impaired quality of life,7,8  increased mortality risk,3,9  higher prevalence of time receiving welfare benefits, and increased risk of leaving the labor market prematurely.9 

In recent years, POCD has received increasing attention in the literature, and several pathophysiologic factors and pathways have been suggested, including anesthetics and inflammatory response to the surgical trauma.10  Animal studies suggest that general anesthetics are a possible pathogenic factor of POCD as brain cell damages, apoptosis, and decrease in cognitive skills are seen in animals after prolonged exposure.11–15  On the basis of the evidence from the human studies, type of surgery is suggested as a risk factor of POCD as the incidence of early POCD after minor surgery was significantly lower than after major surgery.16  Several explanations for this finding have been proposed including nonresolution of the inflammatory response after surgery.10,17  Moreover, the disease prompting the surgery may be responsible for the cognitive impairment as two very common diseases among the elderly, namely cardiac disease and cancer, are found to be associated with cognitive impairment.18–20  A longitudinal study has shown that patients with coronary artery disease showed significant declines in different cognitive domains after 6 yr of follow-up, regardless of whether they had on-pump coronary artery bypass graft surgery, off-pump coronary artery bypass graft surgery, or no surgery.19  Furthermore, the incidence of cognitive impairment in colorectal cancer patients before chemotherapy is more than three times that of aged-matched healthy controls.20  No differences were found by disease stage or between patients pre- and postoperatively, indicating that factors other than surgery and anesthesia were responsible for the higher proportion of cognitive impairment in these patients at baseline.

Mild cognitive impairment with memory deficits has been shown to associate with subsequent dementia, particularly Alzheimer’s disease (AD),21  and it has further been suggested that POCD and dementia share some of the perioperative risk factors, including anesthetics.22  However, a meta-analysis23  including 15 case–control studies found that a history of exposure to general anesthesia was not associated with an increased risk of AD, and also in a recent cohort study, it was not possible to ascertain an association between POCD and dementia.24 

Our aim was to examine the association between exposure to surgery and subsequent level of cognitive functioning in a very large population of middle-aged and elderly twins with almost complete records of surgeries since 1977, hereby enabling us to test for more subtle differences in cognitive functioning. The use of a twin design further enabled us to address the potential confounding from shared environmental and genetic factors as an approach to adjust for preoperative cognitive functioning.

The current study was based on the linkage of the intake examination data of the Longitudinal Study of Middle-Aged Danish Twins (MADT) and the Longitudinal Study of Aging Danish Twins (LSADT) with the Danish National Patient Register (NPR) in Copenhagen. Both studies included an assessment of cognitive functioning and have previously been described in detail elsewhere.25–27  In brief, participants in the MADT and the LSADT were ascertained through the Danish Twin Registry comprising twin pairs born in 1870 to 2004 and considered representative of the general population in Denmark.28,29  The MADT represented a random sample of 120 intact twin pairs from each birth cohort from 1931 to 1952. The LSADT was initiated in 1995 and included Danish twins aged 75 yr or older and residing in Denmark by January 1995. Further cohorts aged at least 70 yr were added in 1997, 1999, and 2001.

The MADT and the LSADT included a questionnaire on sociodemographic factors, health and diseases, and life style factors along with physical and cognitive tests. We excluded patients who had undergone neurosurgery.

Register Linkage

Since 1968, all Danish citizens have been assigned a unique 10-digit identifier, the central personal register number (the CPR number), which is the key to individual information in all official registries covering the Danish population.30  Within Statistics Denmark, the central personal register enables the linkage between Danish registries, including the Danish NPR. The Danish NPR comprises data on surgeries performed in Danish hospitals since 1977 along with data on diagnoses. Until 1996, the Danish National Board of Health’s Classification of Surgeries was used to classify surgeries, and from 1996, the Nordic Classification of Surgical Procedures was used. Data on outpatients and emergency patients were added in 1995.31  Thus, the register has nationwide coverage and is considered to have high validity, especially with regard to surgical procedures.32 

Exposure Assessment

The exposure group encompassed twins who had at least one surgical procedure within 18 to 24 yr before intake cognitive examination. The exposure group was separated into four groups by type of surgical procedure taking into account the severity of the underlying disease: (1) major surgeries, including cardiac, thoracic, laparotomy, central and peripheral vascular, and major fracture surgeries, (2) knee and hip replacement surgeries, (3) minor surgeries, and (4) other surgeries. Minor surgeries included all-day only procedures, surgeries followed by less than 2 days of hospitalization, and, independently of the first two criteria, surgery codes representing eye, skin, endoscopic procedures along with biopsies and other small surgical procedures. The classification was performed by two experienced anesthesiologists (T.G.H. and L.S.R.), who, independently of one another, went through the records of surgery codes in the study sample for the period from 1977 and until intake examination (details can be found in the appendix).

Twins who had more than one surgical procedure were assigned to the groups using the following algorithm: (1) major surgery if any, (2) knee and hip replacement surgery if any, but no major surgery, and (3) other surgeries if any, but neither major nor knee and hip replacement surgery. The reference group in all analyses comprised twins with no surgical procedures from 1977 and until intake examination. The exposure was reassessed by using three more narrow time frames to assess whether time since surgery modified the association between exposure to surgery and cognitive performance: (1) 2 yr (2) 1 yr, and (3) 3 months preceding intake examination. Also the effect of cumulative exposures was investigated based on the type of surgical procedures carried out from 1977 and until cognitive examination. Five exposure groups were defined according to both the type and number of surgeries performed: (1) minor surgeries, (2) one major/other surgery, (3) two major/other surgeries, (4) three major/other surgeries, and (5) more than three major/other surgeries.

Outcome Measurement

The outcome was cognitive functioning assessed by a five-component test battery. The specific tasks included in the test battery were (1) a category fluency task, in which individuals were asked to name as many animals as they could during a 1-min interval, (2) forward and (3) backward digit span, and (4) immediate and (5) delayed recall of a 12-item list. An overall composite measure of cognitive functioning was computed by adding the five standardized scores (using mean and SD from participants aged 45 to 49 yr) of each of the five-component tasks.33  The composite cognitive score has been used in a series of previous studies. It has been shown to be a valid, reliable, and very age-sensitive measure of cognitive functioning although moderately stable over a 2-yr interval (0.60), and it has a high internal consistency reliability (0.75).33–36 

Covariates

Potential confounders were identified in other similar studies,5,37  including sex, age, and educational attainment. However, as the effect sizes were similar regardless of whether educational attainment was included in the statistical model, this covariate was omitted in the analyses.

Intrapair Analyses

To address genetic and shared environmental confounding and to take preoperative cognition into account, intrapair analyses were performed. The 8,503 twins in the study population consisted of 1,065 monozygotic pairs and 1,179 dizygotic, same-sexed twin pairs, as well as 4,015 twin individuals from opposite-sexed pairs and “single twins,” that is twins who had a co-twin who was either deceased or a nonparticipant. To avoid confounding by sex, the intrapair analyses only included same-sexed pairs. To obtain the biggest contrast in exposure, the intrapair analyses only included pairs in whom one had a history of major surgery and the other no surgery. Among the 1,065 monozygotic pairs, a total of 87 such pairs were identified, whereas the corresponding number for dizygotic twins was 124 among 1,179 pairs. Consequently, 211 exposure-discordant pairs were included in the intrapair analyses.

Statistical Analyses

Descriptive statistics were expressed as means and SDs for continuous variables and as numbers and proportions for categories. The comparisons of composite cognitive scores between twins exposed for at least one surgery (major, minor surgery, knee and hip replacement surgery, and other surgeries) before examination of cognitive functioning and twins not exposed were performed using multivariate linear regression models adjusting for sex and age at examination. The within-pair dependency of twin individuals was considered by estimating the standard errors using the cluster option of STATA (release 13; STATA Corp., USA). Model assumptions were checked by residual plots and quantile-quantile plots of residuals.

In the intrapair analyses, the proportion of pairs in which the co-twin who had been exposed to anesthesia and major surgery also had the lower composite cognitive score was calculated including the 95% CI using the binomial distribution.

A total of 8,527 Danish twins with a valid cognitive score were included: 4,309 of 4,314 from the MADT (younger than 70 yr) and 4,218 of 4,731 from the LSADT (70 yr or older).

Of these participants, 65% had at least one surgery preceding intake examination. We excluded 24 twins with neurosurgery from the study: 10 from the MADT and 14 from the LSADT, reducing the sample sizes to 4,299 and 4,204, respectively (table 1).

Table 1.

Characteristics and Summary Statistics for Composite Cognitive Scores among Participants from the Study of Middle-aged Danish Twins (< 70 Yr) and the Longitudinal Study of Aging Danish Twins (≥ 70 Yr), in Totals and by Type of Surgical Procedure

Characteristics and Summary Statistics for Composite Cognitive Scores among Participants from the Study of Middle-aged Danish Twins (< 70 Yr) and the Longitudinal Study of Aging Danish Twins (≥ 70 Yr), in Totals and by Type of Surgical Procedure
Characteristics and Summary Statistics for Composite Cognitive Scores among Participants from the Study of Middle-aged Danish Twins (< 70 Yr) and the Longitudinal Study of Aging Danish Twins (≥ 70 Yr), in Totals and by Type of Surgical Procedure

Major surgery was slightly more common in twins aged 70 yr and older compared with twins younger than 70 yr (20 vs. 17%). The unadjusted mean cognitive scores varied slightly between exposure groups, whereas the cognitive scores were substantially lower for older compared with younger participants, as expected. However, twins aged 70 yr and older with knee and hip replacement surgery had the highest cognitive score (1.68) of surgery groups (ranging from 0.88 to 1.68) and even higher than the reference group (1.24).

Compared with the reference group, twins with major surgery preceding intake examination had slightly lower cognitive scores when adjusted for sex and age (mean difference, −0.27; 95% CI, −0.48 to −0.06) (table 2), which is equivalent to about one tenth of an SD (table 1), that is, negligible effect size.38  In contrast, we found that those with knee and hip replacement surgery had higher cognitive score at intake (mean difference, 0.35; 95% CI, −0.18 to 0.87) although the difference did not reach significance. For twins with minor or other surgical procedures, no differences were found in cognitive score in comparison with the reference group. Analyses stratified on overall age (younger than 70 yr and 70 yr or older) yielded similar estimates.

Table 2.

Mean Differences in Composite Cognitive Scores between Twins with Major, Minor, Knee and Hip Replacement, or Other Surgical Procedures and the Reference Group

Mean Differences in Composite Cognitive Scores between Twins with Major, Minor, Knee and Hip Replacement, or Other Surgical Procedures and the Reference Group
Mean Differences in Composite Cognitive Scores between Twins with Major, Minor, Knee and Hip Replacement, or Other Surgical Procedures and the Reference Group

To further illustrate the effect size associated with major surgery (0.1 SD), we analyzed separately one of the cognitive tests included in the cognitive composite score, namely the animal fluency test (i.e., mention as many animals as possible in 1 min). Here, one tenth of an SD corresponds to 0.7 named animal. In the MADT, the mean number of animals named in 1 min is 24.2, and this means that a one tenth of an SD lower score corresponds to 23.5, which represents a difference that must be considered clinically unimportant. In comparison, the difference in cognitive score between the group younger than 70 yr and that older than 70 yr is about a 0.9 SD (table 1), which in the animal fluency test would correspond to a six-word difference.

We also analyzed the data according to those who had undergone surgery up to 2 yr (n = 1,738), 1 yr (n = 1,109), or 3 months (n = 391), respectively, preceding intake examination (fig. 1). Overall, the mean differences in cognitive scores in the surgery groups compared with those without surgery in the same periods were similar across strata. Although the majority of estimates did not reveal statistical significance, the results indicated no interaction of time since surgery with the association between surgery and cognitive score at intake examination. When we stratified on overall age (younger than 70 yr and 70 yr or older), the analyses yielded similar estimates in each of the age groups.

Fig. 1.

Mean differences in composite cognitive scores between twins who had undergone major, minor, knee and hip replacement, or other surgical procedures up to 24 yr (n = 5,538), 2 yr (n = 1,738), 1 yr (n = 1,109), or 3 months (n = 391) preceding intake examination and the nonexposed group; (A) all participants; (B) participants aged < 70 yr; (C) participants aged ≥ 70 yr.

Fig. 1.

Mean differences in composite cognitive scores between twins who had undergone major, minor, knee and hip replacement, or other surgical procedures up to 24 yr (n = 5,538), 2 yr (n = 1,738), 1 yr (n = 1,109), or 3 months (n = 391) preceding intake examination and the nonexposed group; (A) all participants; (B) participants aged < 70 yr; (C) participants aged ≥ 70 yr.

Close modal

In addition, analyses were stratified on the type and number of surgical procedures to investigate the dose–response pattern of cumulative exposure to surgery and anesthesia on cognitive impairment (table 3). We found that exposure to three or more surgeries was associated with significantly lower score, about one fifth of an SD.

Table 3.

Mean Differences in Composite Cognitive Scores between Twins with Minor Surgery, One Major/Other Surgery, Two Major/Other Surgeries, Three Major/Other Surgeries or More Than Three Major/Other Surgeries and the Group without Surgery Preceding Intake Examination

Mean Differences in Composite Cognitive Scores between Twins with Minor Surgery, One Major/Other Surgery, Two Major/Other Surgeries, Three Major/Other Surgeries or More Than Three Major/Other Surgeries and the Group without Surgery Preceding Intake Examination
Mean Differences in Composite Cognitive Scores between Twins with Minor Surgery, One Major/Other Surgery, Two Major/Other Surgeries, Three Major/Other Surgeries or More Than Three Major/Other Surgeries and the Group without Surgery Preceding Intake Examination

The intrapair analysis of twin pairs in whom one had a history of major surgery and the other had no surgery was included to further adjust for genetic and shared environmental factors. In the analysis of all 211 discordant pairs, we found that the surgery-exposed twin had a lower cognitive score than the co-twin in 49% (95% CI, 42 to 56%) of the pairs (fig. 2), suggesting that it was as common to have the higher cognitive score when exposed to surgery as it was to have the lower cognitive score. When twin pairs were restricted according to the magnitude of the intrapair differences in cognitive score to the top 75, 50, and 25%, respectively, the analyses yielded similar results. This was also the case when the twins were stratified by zygosity (data not shown).

Fig. 2.

Proportion of twin pairs in which the twin exposed to major surgery had a lower composite cognitive score for all twin pairs and stratified according to the magnitude of intrapair difference in cognitive score. Above each line is the P value from the binomial test, which tests whether the proportion equals to 0.5.

Fig. 2.

Proportion of twin pairs in which the twin exposed to major surgery had a lower composite cognitive score for all twin pairs and stratified according to the magnitude of intrapair difference in cognitive score. Above each line is the P value from the binomial test, which tests whether the proportion equals to 0.5.

Close modal

In this study comprising 8,503 twins, major surgery was associated with a negligibly lower level of cognitive functioning. Thus, the study provides evidence against a clinically relevant association between exposure to surgical procedures and anesthesia and subsequent cognition in mid- and late life.

Twins who had undergone major surgery had slightly lower cognitive scores, corresponding to one tenth of an SD, compared with the reference group, but in the intrapair analysis where genetic and shared environmental factors were adjusted for by design, no association was observed. Twins who had undergone hip or knee replacement had slightly higher cognitive scores, although not significantly so, whereas no differences were found in the minor or other surgery group when compared with the reference group.

The time interval between surgery and cognitive examination had no impact on effect sizes, and no dose–response pattern for number of surgeries was found although having had three or more major/other surgeries was significantly associated with a lower cognitive score corresponding to one fifth of an SD.

Our results are reassuring in relation to long-term adverse cognitive effects of exposure to major surgery and anesthesia in adulthood for two reasons: First, effect sizes of one tenth to one fifth of an SD are very subtle and may be clinically irrelevant; second, our large sample size ensures estimates with high confidence. Cognitive impairment is a feared aspect of aging,39  and thus, our finding of a subtle yet persistent cognitive impairment after surgery and anesthesia may be a concern. However, the results from the intrapair analysis and across surgical procedures suggest that factors other than surgery and anesthesia are important. In the intrapair analysis, the association between major surgery/anesthesia and cognitive score disappeared, and along with evidence of high correlation in cognitive scores among twins,40  this suggest that long-term postoperative cognitive functioning is primarily determined by the preoperative cognitive functioning. The severity of the underlying diseases across surgery groups is diverse with the more severe diseases in twins belonging to the major surgery group (e.g., cardiac and vascular surgeries and laparotomies) than to the minor (e.g., eye and laparoscopic surgeries) and knee and hip replacement surgery groups. Thus, the observations of a lower cognitive score in the major surgery group, no difference in the minor and other surgery groups, and even a higher score in the hip and knee replacement group in comparison with the reference group indicate that the underlying disease is a major determining factor.

There was a tendency to better cognitive scores in the hip and knee replacement group compared with all other groups including the reference group. This is most likely due to the fact that the best functioning individuals are offered hip and knee replacements. Furthermore, it could also be explained by reduction in pain and increased mobility after successful joint replacements and subsequent improvement in level of functioning.

Comparisons with other studies are hampered by differences in methodology, including differences in design, endpoints, and length of follow-up. The current study reports mean differences in composite cognitive score, whereas most other studies report dichotomous outcomes, primarily incidence of POCD. Moreover, the current study has longer follow-up (up to 24 yr) than other studies.41 

Despite the methodological differences, our findings across surgical procedures and in the intrapair analyses are to a large extent in line with other studies that assessed cognition more than 6 months postoperatively.16,37,42  Still, a recent cohort study43  (n = 192) found an increased incidence of POCD in surgical patients older than 60 yr compared with age- and sex-matched controls at 1 yr postoperatively, and a meta-analysis of 17 studies44  concluded that no impairment in cognitive performance could be documented at 3 to 6 months after hip and knee replacement. However, only two small studies that included control groups assessed cognitive functioning at 3 months of follow-up in this meta-analysis and that conclusion may, therefore, not be justified. Another study of 270 elderly patients undergoing elective orthopedic surgery found improvements in some verbal cognitive tasks including immediate and delayed verbal recall and either deterioration or no differences in other tasks at follow-up at approximately 1 yr.45  Hence, our findings of a tendency to a slightly higher composite cognitive score in the hip or knee replacement group could be considered to be in agreement with these findings. However, caution in this interpretation is warranted as the effect size in our study is small and nonsignificant. In addition, even if surgery successfully treats chronic pain and restores both physical and cognitive functioning in this patient group,46,47  the recovery may take time and is only registered by longer follow-up.

Our finding of no effect of time interval between surgery and cognitive examination on effect sizes does not support the current evidence of a gradual resolution of POCD between 3 months and 1 to 2 yr,6,37  but the CIs are quite wide.

Exposure to three or more surgeries seemed to be a risk factor but may be explained by underlying disease(s) or complications after surgery. Interestingly, a cross-sectional study48  found no association between number of general anesthetic exposures and cognitive deficits in 606 participants aged 75 yr or older. Likewise, a case–control study of 1,010 participants found no association between cumulative number of exposures of general anesthesia and AD.49 

Major strengths of the current study are the use of study samples from population-based surveys retrieved from the Danish Twin Registry28  and the completeness of the Danish NPR,31,32  which minimized the risk for selection bias. The Danish National Health Care System with no out-of-pocket expenses further minimizes the bias from different access to health care depending on the educational and socioeconomic factors that are known to be strongly associated with cognitive performance. However, the study is performed in a relatively homogeneous Northern European population and thus the results should be replicated in other settings.

The large sample size of more than 8,500 twins is another strength as this enabled us to detect even very subtle cognitive score differences with high accuracy. In the literature, there is a compelling body of evidence that twins are similar to the general population with regard to health and survival50–54  and in recent cohorts also with regard to cognitive functioning.55  An observed, slightly lower cognitive functioning in older cohorts56  should not affect the associations in this study as the comparisons are conducted within a twin population. In addition, we have no reason to believe that the effect of surgery and anesthesia differs between twins and singletons and so our findings are likely generalizable to the nontwin population.

The use of the co-twin design has a number of strengths. First, it is statistically powerful, reflected in the narrow CI on the effect measure (proportion of pairs in which the major surgery-exposed twin had the lowest score: 49%; 95% CI, 42 to 56%) despite that only 211 pairs were included. Furthermore, the design controls for childhood socioeconomic status and genetic endowment, both known to be strongly associated with cognitive functioning in mid- and late life.34,57  Finally, due to the high correlation in cognitive performance within twin pairs, especially within monozygotic twin pairs, the co-twin is the best matched control.

Even though we argued that the risk of selection bias is small in this study, selection bias due to mortality and nonattendance should be considered when interpreting the results. The nonattendance of 518 twins (6%) in the cognitive examination may be explained by severe cognitive or physical impairments.36  An explorative data analysis (not shown) revealed that nonattendees had 1.3 times higher odds of having had any surgery than attendees. Hence, it cannot be ruled out that a small fraction of surgery-exposed twins may be particularly susceptible to surgery and anesthesia or disadvantaged in other ways.

The cross-sectional design of this study did not allow us to consider selective mortality, that is, those with cognitive decline may have less chance of surviving to the study intake. Results were similar when restricting the analyses to surgeries performed less than 3 months before intake and hence minimizing the risk of survival bias. We did not assess cognition preceding surgery/anesthesia. However, in the intrapair analysis, preoperative cognition was taken into account to some degree as the intrapair similarity of intelligence is high.40 

Unmeasured comorbidity is likely to represent an important factor for cognitive performance. If the operated participants have less comorbidity than the nonoperated (which may be anticipated among the oldest in the cohort because surgery is often not performed among those who are very frail), the surgery–cognition association will be underestimated. However, if the operated participants have more comorbidity (which may be anticipated among middle-aged and younger elderly), the association will be overestimated. We see no such age dependence in our study (table 2). A limitation of the exposure measure is that information about outpatient procedures performed before 1995 is not available. This could mean inclusion of exposed cohort members in the unexposed group and tend to bias the results toward null. However, major surgery very rarely took place in an outpatient setting before 1995.

Finally, alternative explanations related to the cognitive measure used in this study should be considered in the interpretation of our findings. Low sensitivity may be associated with an inability to detect cognitive deficits that may have been picked up by other cognitive test batteries. It is noteworthy that the composite cognitive score used is shown to be very age sensitive with a difference of 2.5 SD from age 45 to 90 yr.33  Although the composite cognitive score does not capture exactly the same cognitive domains as the psychometric test batteries used to detect POCD, it reflects elements that are pivotal for daily living, including working memory.

In conclusion, exposure to major surgery and anesthesia was associated with a negligible reduction in cognitive functioning among middle-aged and elderly twins. This finding substantiates the current evidence of no long-term major adverse cognitive effect of surgery and anesthesia as the very large sample size along with the sensitive cognitive measure allow us to detect very subtle effect sizes.

The Danish Twin Registry and the Danish Aging Research Center have received grants from the National Program for Research Infrastructure, the Danish Agency for Science Technology and Innovation, Copenhagen, Denmark, 2007 (grant no. 09-063256); the VELUX Foundation, Copenhagen, Denmark; and the U.S. National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Maryland (grant no. P01 AG08761). Dr. Rasmussen has received funding from the Tryg Foundation, Virum, Denmark. The funding sources were involved neither in the design, analysis, and preparation of the article nor in the decision to submit the article for publication.

The authors declare no competing interests.

1.
Krenk
L
,
Kehlet
H
,
Bæk Hansen
T
,
Solgaard
S
,
Soballe
K
,
Rasmussen
LS
:
Cognitive dysfunction after fast-track hip and knee replacement.
Anesth Analg
2014
;
118
:
1034
40
2.
Johnson
T
,
Monk
T
,
Rasmussen
LS
,
Abildstrom
H
,
Houx
P
,
Korttila
K
,
Kuipers
HM
,
Hanning
CD
,
Siersma
VD
,
Kristensen
D
,
Canet
J
,
Ibañaz
MT
,
Moller
JT
;
ISPOCD2 Investigators
:
Postoperative cognitive dysfunction in middle-aged patients.
Anesthesiology
2002
;
96
:
1351
7
3.
Monk
TG
,
Weldon
BC
,
Garvan
CW
,
Dede
DE
,
van der Aa
MT
,
Heilman
KM
,
Gravenstein
JS
:
Predictors of cognitive dysfunction after major noncardiac surgery.
Anesthesiology
2008
;
108
:
18
30
4.
Rasmussen
LS
:
Postoperative cognitive dysfunction: Incidence and prevention.
Best Pract Res Clin Anaesthesiol
2006
;
20
:
315
30
5.
Moller
JT
,
Cluitmans
P
,
Rasmussen
LS
,
Houx
P
,
Rasmussen
H
,
Canet
J
,
Rabbitt
P
,
Jolles
J
,
Larsen
K
,
Hanning
CD
,
Langeron
O
,
Johnson
T
,
Lauven
PM
,
Kristensen
PA
,
Biedler
A
,
van Beem
H
,
Fraidakis
O
,
Silverstein
JH
,
Beneken
JE
,
Gravenstein
JS
:
Long-term postoperative cognitive dysfunction in the elderly ISPOCD1 study. ISPOCD investigators. International Study of Post-Operative Cognitive Dysfunction.
Lancet
1998
;
351
:
857
61
6.
Abildstrom
H
,
Rasmussen
LS
,
Rentowl
P
,
Hanning
CD
,
Rasmussen
H
,
Kristensen
PA
,
Moller
JT
:
Cognitive dysfunction 1–2 years after non-cardiac surgery in the elderly. ISPOCD group. International Study of Post-Operative Cognitive Dysfunction.
Acta Anaesthesiol Scand
2000
;
44
:
1246
51
7.
Newman
MF
,
Grocott
HP
,
Mathew
JP
,
White
WD
,
Landolfo
K
,
Reves
JG
,
Laskowitz
DT
,
Mark
DB
,
Blumenthal
JA
;
Neurologic Outcome Research Group and the Cardiothoracic Anesthesia Research Endeavors (CARE) Investigators of the Duke Heart Center
:
Report of the substudy assessing the impact of neurocognitive function on quality of life 5 years after cardiac surgery.
Stroke
2001
;
32
:
2874
81
8.
Phillips-Bute
B
,
Mathew
JP
,
Blumenthal
JA
,
Grocott
HP
,
Laskowitz
DT
,
Jones
RH
,
Mark
DB
,
Newman
MF
:
Association of neurocognitive function and quality of life 1 year after coronary artery bypass graft (CABG) surgery.
Psychosom Med
2006
;
68
:
369
75
9.
Steinmetz
J
,
Christensen
KB
,
Lund
T
,
Lohse
N
,
Rasmussen
LS
;
ISPOCD Group
:
Long-term consequences of postoperative cognitive dysfunction.
Anesthesiology
2009
;
110
:
548
55
10.
Krenk
L
,
Rasmussen
LS
,
Kehlet
H
:
New insights into the pathophysiology of postoperative cognitive dysfunction.
Acta Anaesthesiol Scand
2010
;
54
:
951
6
11.
Culley
DJ
,
Baxter
MG
,
Crosby
CA
,
Yukhananov
R
,
Crosby
G
:
Impaired acquisition of spatial memory 2 weeks after isoflurane and isoflurane-nitrous oxide anesthesia in aged rats.
Anesth Analg
2004
;
99
:
1393
7
12.
Culley
DJ
,
Baxter
MG
,
Yukhananov
R
,
Crosby
G
:
Long-term impairment of acquisition of a spatial memory task following isoflurane-nitrous oxide anesthesia in rats.
Anesthesiology
2004
;
100
:
309
14
13.
Xie
Z
,
Culley
DJ
,
Dong
Y
,
Zhang
G
,
Zhang
B
,
Moir
RD
,
Frosch
MP
,
Crosby
G
,
Tanzi
RE
:
The common inhalation anesthetic isoflurane induces caspase activation and increases amyloid β-protein level in vivo.
Ann Neurol
2008
;
64
:
618
27
14.
Jevtovic-Todorovic
V
,
Beals
J
,
Benshoff
N
,
Olney
JW
:
Prolonged exposure to inhalational anesthetic nitrous oxide kills neurons in adult rat brain.
Neuroscience
2003
;
122
:
609
16
15.
Jevtovic-Todorovic
V
,
Absalom
AR
,
Blomgren
K
,
Brambrink
A
,
Crosby
G
,
Culley
DJ
,
Fiskum
G
,
Giffard
RG
,
Herold
KF
,
Loepke
AW
,
Ma
D
,
Orser
BA
,
Planel
E
,
Slikker
W
Jr
,
Soriano
SG
,
Stratmann
G
,
Vutskits
L
,
Xie
Z
,
Hemmings
HC
Jr
:
Anaesthetic neurotoxicity and neuroplasticity: An expert group report and statement based on the BJA Salzburg Seminar.
Br J Anaesth
2013
;
111
:
143
51
16.
Canet
J
,
Raeder
J
,
Rasmussen
LS
,
Enlund
M
,
Kuipers
HM
,
Hanning
CD
,
Jolles
J
,
Korttila
K
,
Siersma
VD
,
Dodds
C
,
Abildstrom
H
,
Sneyd
JR
,
Vila
P
,
Johnson
T
,
Muñoz Corsini
L
,
Silverstein
JH
,
Nielsen
IK
,
Moller
JT
;
ISPOCD2 Investigators
:
Cognitive dysfunction after minor surgery in the elderly.
Acta Anaesthesiol Scand
2003
;
47
:
1204
10
17.
Vacas
S
,
Degos
V
,
Feng
X
,
Maze
M
:
The neuroinflammatory response of postoperative cognitive decline.
Br Med Bull
2013
;
106
:
161
78
18.
Eggermont
LH
,
de Boer
K
,
Muller
M
,
Jaschke
AC
,
Kamp
O
,
Scherder
EJ
:
Cardiac disease and cognitive impairment: A systematic review.
Heart
2012
;
98
:
1334
40
19.
Selnes
OA
,
Grega
MA
,
Bailey
MM
,
Pham
LD
,
Zeger
SL
,
Baumgartner
WA
,
McKhann
GM
:
Do management strategies for coronary artery disease influence 6-year cognitive outcomes?
Ann Thorac Surg
2009
;
88
:
445
54
20.
Vardy
J
,
Dhillon
HM
,
Pond
GR
,
Rourke
SB
,
Xu
W
,
Dodd
A
,
Renton
C
,
Park
A
,
Bekele
T
,
Ringash
J
,
Zhang
H
,
Burkes
R
,
Clarke
SJ
,
Tannock
IF
:
Cognitive function and fatigue after diagnosis of colorectal cancer.
Ann Oncol
2014
;
25
:
2404
12
21.
Gauthier
S
,
Reisberg
B
,
Zaudig
M
,
Petersen
RC
,
Ritchie
K
,
Broich
K
,
Belleville
S
,
Brodaty
H
,
Bennett
D
,
Chertkow
H
,
Cummings
JL
,
de Leon
M
,
Feldman
H
,
Ganguli
M
,
Hampel
H
,
Scheltens
P
,
Tierney
MC
,
Whitehouse
P
,
Winblad
B
;
International Psychogeriatric Association Expert Conference on Mild Cognitive Impairment
:
Mild cognitive impairment.
Lancet
2006
;
367
:
1262
70
22.
Bilotta
F
,
Doronzio
A
,
Stazi
E
,
Titi
L
,
Fodale
V
,
Di Nino
G
,
Rosa
G
:
Postoperative cognitive dysfunction: Toward the Alzheimer’s disease pathomechanism hypothesis.
J Alzheimers Dis
2010
;
22
(
suppl 3
):
81
9
23.
Seitz
DP
,
Shah
PS
,
Herrmann
N
,
Beyene
J
,
Siddiqui
N
:
Exposure to general anesthesia and risk of Alzheimer’s disease: A systematic review and meta-analysis.
BMC Geriatr
2011
;
11
:
83
24.
Steinmetz
J
,
Siersma
V
,
Kessing
LV
,
Rasmussen
LS
;
ISPOCD Group
:
Is postoperative cognitive dysfunction a risk factor for dementia? A cohort follow-up study.
Br J Anaesth
2013
;
110
(
suppl 1
):
i92
7
25.
Christensen
K
,
Holm
NV
,
McGue
M
,
Corder
L
,
Vaupel
JW
:
A Danish population-based twin study on general health in the elderly.
J Aging Health
1999
;
11
:
49
64
26.
Gaist
D
,
Bathum
L
,
Skytthe
A
,
Jensen
TK
,
McGue
M
,
Vaupel
JW
,
Christensen
K
:
Strength and anthropometric measures in identical and fraternal twins: No evidence of masculinization of females with male co-twins.
Epidemiology
2000
;
11
:
340
3
27.
Nybo
H
,
Gaist
D
,
Jeune
B
,
Bathum
L
,
McGue
M
,
Vaupel
JW
,
Christensen
K
:
The Danish 1905 cohort: A genetic-epidemiological nationwide survey.
J Aging Health
2001
;
13
:
32
46
28.
Skytthe
A
,
Christiansen
L
,
Kyvik
KO
,
Bødker
FL
,
Hvidberg
L
,
Petersen
I
,
Nielsen
MM
,
Bingley
P
,
Hjelmborg
J
,
Tan
Q
,
Holm
NV
,
Vaupel
JW
,
McGue
M
,
Christensen
K
:
The Danish Twin Registry: Linking surveys, national registers, and biological information.
Twin Res Hum Genet
2013
;
16
:
104
11
29.
Skytthe
A
,
Kyvik
K
,
Holm
NV
,
Vaupel
JW
,
Christensen
K
:
The Danish Twin Registry: 127 birth cohorts of twins.
Twin Res
2002
;
5
:
352
7
30.
Pedersen
CB
:
The Danish Civil Registration System.
Scand J Public Health
2011
;
39
(
7 suppl
):
22
5
31.
Lynge
E
,
Sandegaard
JL
,
Rebolj
M
:
The Danish National Patient Register.
Scand J Public Health
2011
;
39
(
7 suppl
):
30
3
32.
Andersen
TF
,
Madsen
M
,
Jørgensen
J
,
Mellemkjoer
L
,
Olsen
JH
:
The Danish National Hospital Register. A valuable source of data for modern health sciences.
Dan Med Bull
1999
;
46
:
263
8
33.
Vestergaard
S
,
Thinggaard
M
,
Jeune
B
,
Vaupel
JW
,
McGue
M
,
Christensen
K
:
Physical and mental decline and yet rather happy? A study of Danes aged 45 and older.
Aging Ment Health
2015
;
19
:
400
8
34.
McGue
M
,
Christensen
K
:
Growing old but not growing apart: Twin similarity in the latter half of the lifespan.
Behav Genet
2013
;
43
:
1
12
35.
McGue
M
,
Christensen
K
:
The heritability of cognitive functioning in very old adults: Evidence from Danish twins aged 75 years and older.
Psychol Aging
2001
;
16
:
272
80
36.
McGue
M
,
Christensen
K
:
The heritability of level and rate-of-change in cognitive functioning in Danish twins aged 70 years and older.
Exp Aging Res
2002
;
28
:
435
51
37.
Newman
S
,
Stygall
J
,
Hirani
S
,
Shaefi
S
,
Maze
M
:
Postoperative cognitive dysfunction after noncardiac surgery: A systematic review.
Anesthesiology
2007
;
106
:
572
90
38.
Cohen
J
:
Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences
, 2nd edition.
Hillsdale
,
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
,
1988
39.
Martin
GM
:
Defeating dementia.
Nature
2004
;
431
:
247
8
40.
Deary
IJ
,
Johnson
W
,
Houlihan
LM
:
Genetic foundations of human intelligence.
Hum Genet
2009
;
126
:
215
32
41.
Avidan
MS
,
Searleman
AC
,
Storandt
M
,
Barnett
K
,
Vannucci
A
,
Saager
L
,
Xiong
C
,
Grant
EA
,
Kaiser
D
,
Morris
JC
,
Evers
AS
:
Long-term cognitive decline in older subjects was not attributable to noncardiac surgery or major illness.
Anesthesiology
2009
;
111
:
964
70
42.
Mashour
GA
,
Woodrum
DT
,
Avidan
MS
:
Neurological complications of surgery and anaesthesia.
Br J Anaesth
2015
;
114
:
194
203
43.
Ballard
C
,
Jones
E
,
Gauge
N
,
Aarsland
D
,
Nilsen
OB
,
Saxby
BK
,
Lowery
D
,
Corbett
A
,
Wesnes
K
,
Katsaiti
E
,
Arden
J
,
Amoako
D
,
Amaoko
D
,
Prophet
N
,
Purushothaman
B
,
Green
D
:
Optimised anaesthesia to reduce post operative cognitive decline (POCD) in older patients undergoing elective surgery, a randomised controlled trial.
PLoS One
2012
;
7
:
e37410
44.
Scott
JE
,
Mathias
JL
,
Kneebone
AC
:
Postoperative cognitive dysfunction after total joint arthroplasty in the elderly: A meta-analysis.
J Arthroplasty
2014
;
29
:
261
7.e1
45.
Ancelin
ML
,
de Roquefeuil
G
,
Scali
J
,
Bonnel
F
,
Adam
JF
,
Cheminal
JC
,
Cristol
JP
,
Dupuy
AM
,
Carrière
I
,
Ritchie
K
:
Long-term post-operative cognitive decline in the elderly: The effects of anesthesia type, apolipoprotein E genotype, and clinical antecedents.
J Alzheimers Dis
2010
;
22
(
suppl 3
):
105
13
46.
Rodriguez-Raecke
R
,
Niemeier
A
,
Ihle
K
,
Ruether
W
,
May
A
:
Brain gray matter decrease in chronic pain is the consequence and not the cause of pain.
J Neurosci
2009
;
29
:
13746
50
47.
Seminowicz
DA
,
Wideman
TH
,
Naso
L
,
Hatami-Khoroushahi
Z
,
Fallatah
S
,
Ware
MA
,
Jarzem
P
,
Bushnell
MC
,
Shir
Y
,
Ouellet
JA
,
Stone
LS
:
Effective treatment of chronic low back pain in humans reverses abnormal brain anatomy and function.
J Neurosci
2011
;
31
:
7540
50
48.
Fischer
P
,
Wallner
H
,
Jungwirth
S
,
Weissgram
S
,
Krampla
W
,
Bauer
P
,
Tragl
KH
:
Cumulative Exposure to General anesthesias and cognitive dysfunction at age 75 in the Vienna Transdanube Aging “VITA” study.
J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci
2007
;
19
:
21
6
49.
Bohnen
NI
,
Warner
MA
,
Kokmen
E
,
Beard
CM
,
Kurland
LT
:
Alzheimer’s disease and cumulative exposure to anesthesia: A case-control study.
J Am Geriatr Soc
1994
;
42
:
198
201
50.
Petersen
I
,
Nielsen
MM
,
Beck-Nielsen
H
,
Christensen
K
:
No evidence of a higher 10 year period prevalence of diabetes among 77,885 twins compared with 215,264 singletons from the Danish birth cohorts 1910–1989.
Diabetologia
2011
;
54
:
2016
24
51.
Simmons
SF
,
Johansson
B
,
Zarit
SH
,
Ljungquist
B
,
Plomin
R
,
McClearn
GE
:
Selection bias in samples of older twins? A comparison between octogenarian twins and singletons in Sweden.
J Aging Health
1997
;
9
:
553
67
52.
Öberg
S
,
Cnattingius
S
,
Sandin
S
,
Lichtenstein
P
,
Morley
R
,
Iliadou
AN
:
Twinship influence on morbidity and mortality across the lifespan.
Int J Epidemiol
2012
;
41
:
1002
9
53.
Christensen
K
,
Vaupel
JW
,
Holm
NV
,
Yashin
AI
:
Mortality among twins after age 6: Fetal origins hypothesis versus twin method.
BMJ
1995
;
310
:
432
6
54.
Christensen
K
,
Wienke
A
,
Skytthe
A
,
Holm
NV
,
Vaupel
JW
,
Yashin
AI
:
Cardiovascular mortality in twins and the fetal origins hypothesis.
Twin Res
2001
;
4
:
344
9
55.
Christensen
K
,
Petersen
I
,
Skytthe
A
,
Herskind
AM
,
McGue
M
,
Bingley
P
:
Comparison of academic performance of twins and singletons in adolescence: Follow-up study.
BMJ
2006
;
333
:
1095
56.
Ronalds
GA
,
De Stavola
BL
,
Leon
DA
:
The cognitive cost of being a twin: Evidence from comparisons within families in the Aberdeen children of the 1950s cohort study.
BMJ
2005
;
331
:
1306
57.
Fors
S
,
Lennartsson
C
,
Lundberg
O
:
Childhood living conditions, socioeconomic position in adulthood, and cognition in later life: Exploring the associations.
J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci
2009
;
64
:
750
7

Surgery Codes According to the Danish National Board of Health’s Classification of Surgeries and Treatments and the Nordic Classification of Surgical Procedures That Are Included in the Specified Surgery Groups: Major Surgery, Minor Surgery, Knee and Hip Replacement Surgery, and Other Surgeries