To the Editor:

Dr. Perouansky’s insightful and provocative retrospective about Claude Bernard’s influence on theories of general anesthesia1  was a delightful read, as was the accompanying editorial.2  The article presented an interesting mix of metaphors, at first alluding to Claude Bernard’s “long shadow,” and ending with a warning against only “searching where the light is bright.” In addition, Dr. Perouansky discussed Bernard’s influence in terms of a long-lived Kuhnian paradigm, implying that Bernard’s influence persists, without concluding whether the paradigm has indeed shifted, and if so, what catalyzed this change. Here, I introduce another metaphor to illustrate the concept of “paradigm shift” and other perspectives on this question.

An illustrative metaphor for a scientific paradigm is that of an ever-expanding underground mine, where veins of knowledge are explored and illuminated by scientific mine workers. Kuhnian “normal science” expands and elaborates established veins within the mine, extracting an ever-increasing collection of knowledge, while also increasing the area to be explored (the walls bounding the mine tunnels). The material surrounding the mine walls represents nascent knowledge, mapped to varying degrees by scientific theory. The essential scientific tools are imagination and experimental methods (along with people and funding). As with real mining for valuable ore or gemstones, different veins yield at different rates and to different tools (depending on the hardness of the region). Some veins expand to reveal new side veins, attracting more scientists. In contrast, some veins stop yielding after a period of exploration, when ideas are disproven or methods fail. Kuhnian “paradigm shifts” markedly increase the extent of this scientific mine and, more importantly, change where scientists choose to explore. Paradigm shifts may be actively sought when scientific ideas are failing, indicating the need for new theoretical directions. Sometimes a new theory emerges suddenly, because a connection to another scientific discipline is made, guiding exploration in a new expansive direction. Paradigm shifts may also be triggered by new scientific methods that enable “breakthroughs” in previously impenetrable scientific barriers. In this context, I believe that a paradigm shift has indeed occurred in the science of general anesthesia and that it was multifactorial.

Karl Popper3  defined a viable scientific hypothesis as a postulate that can be experimentally falsified. As Dr. Perouansky emphasizes, the studies of Meyer and Overton successfully elaborated on Bernard’s “unitary” framework by focusing on lipids. A variety of more detailed lipid hypotheses ensued, each focused on different biophysical properties of lipids. These hypotheses were experimentally based, yet none met Popper’s criteria of falsifiability. Yes, exceptions to the Meyer–Overton correlation were found, and mostly set aside without full explanation or exploration, but experimental model systems that might definitively disprove the role of lipids in anesthesia did not exist when these theories were formulated, and for the most part, they are still lacking. Thus, some lipid hypotheses remain viable (although neglected), although Popper would deem these hypotheses “unscientific” until we develop new approaches to test them. Metaphorically, this is blasting away at soft parts of the wall, although critical knowledge is only in the hard parts, for which we have few tools to excavate.

Conceptual and experimental advances in the broader scientific fields of biophysics, molecular biology, and neurobiology also influenced theories of general anesthesia. Franks and Lieb4  launched their assault on lipid theories using then new biophysical tools (neutron diffraction) that accurately measured effects of clinical anesthetic concentrations on the structure of lipid bilayers. Although Franks and Lieb4  may not have fully envisioned the paradigm shift to come, they deserve credit for leading the field away from a barren area (lipids) toward richer scientific territory (protein). This shift was facilitated by concurrent developments in molecular biology. Molecular tools enabled scientists to insert mutations into suspected anesthetic binding sites on potential target proteins (“reverse pharmacology”), including neuronal ion channels, and eventually to alter anesthetic target proteins in transgenic animals (“knock-ins”). These were hard experiments but provided definitive advances in understanding anesthetic mechanisms and helped further undermine unitary concepts.

Dr. Perouansky touched briefly on a foundational issue that, in my opinion, truly heralds the occurrence of a paradigm shift in our field: redefining general anesthesia. Embedded within the unitary hypothesis was the idea that general anesthesia is a singular state transition, from irritable to unresponsive, even to noxious stimuli (minimum alveolar concentration-immobility). The distinct concept of minimum alveolar concentration-awake,5  together with clinical evidence that perceptive awareness and memory formation can occur in anesthetized immobile patients, led both clinicians and researchers to a more nuanced and neurobiological framework for understanding and studying general anesthesia. It is now widely accepted that anesthetics produce distinct neurobehavioral (and clinically relevant) endpoints via actions on separate neural circuits and networks. Furthermore, distinct classes of anesthetics can be recognized based on relative potencies for clinical endpoints, correlating with activity at distinct sets of molecular targets.6,7  These observations defy unitary concepts at multiple system levels.

In summary, the last few decades have been a period of maturation and diversification in research on general anesthesia, and there is little room now for 19th century thinking, despite the seductive elegance of Claude Bernard’s ideas. Although the Meyer–Overton correlation has not been expunged from textbooks, and the biophysical concept of hydrophobicity remains important to anesthetic pharmacology, these ideas have been subsumed into a broader neurobiological framework. Research investigating unitary mechanisms or even alluding to these mechanisms in discussions of new experiments is diminishing. Are unitary hypotheses dead? Not quite. Many ideas central to unitary hypotheses have collapsed, and few scientists dig there, but some tunnels of knowledge in that area remain surrounded by hard material that resists excavation. Instead, research on mechanisms of anesthesia is emerging as a field of systems neurobiology, linked closely to research on mechanisms of consciousness.8–10  Moreover, advances in our scientific understanding of general anesthesia are starting to translate into promising efforts to develop better general anesthetic drugs11  and improved monitors for assessing depth of anesthesia.12 

1.
Perouansky
M
:
The quest for a unified model of anesthetic action: A century in Claude Bernard’s shadow.
Anesthesiology
2012
;
117
:
465
74
2.
Ginosar
Y
,
Binshtok
AM
:
Mechanisms in anesthesia and analgesia: Convention, crisis, and the shoulders of giants.
Anesthesiology
2012
;
117
:
451
3
3.
Popper
K
:
Logik der Forschung Zur Erkenntnistheorie Der Modernen Naturwissenschaft (the Logic of Scientific Rresearch)
.
Tuebingen, Germany
,
Mohr Siebeck
,
1935
4.
Franks
NP
,
Lieb
WR
:
Where do general anaesthetics act?
Nature
1978
;
274
:
339
42
5.
Stoelting
RK
,
Longnecker
DE
,
Eger
EI
2nd
:
Minimum alveolar concentrations in man on awakening from methoxyflurane, halothane, ether and fluroxene anesthesia: MAC awake.
Anesthesiology
1970
;
33
:
5
9
6.
Grasshoff
C
,
Rudolph
U
,
Antkowiak
B
:
Molecular and systemic mechanisms of general anaesthesia: The ‘multi-site and multiple mechanisms’ concept.
Curr Opin Anaesthesiol
2005
;
18
:
386
91
7.
Solt
K
,
Forman
SA
:
Correlating the clinical actions and molecular mechanisms of general anesthetics.
Curr Opin Anaesthesiol
2007
;
20
:
300
6
8.
Mashour
GA
:
Consciousness unbound: Toward a paradigm of general anesthesia.
Anesthesiology
2004
;
100
:
428
33
9.
Alkire
MT
,
Hudetz
AG
,
Tononi
G
:
Consciousness and anesthesia.
Science
2008
;
322
:
876
80
10.
Brown
EN
,
Purdon
PL
,
Van Dort
CJ
:
General anesthesia and altered states of arousal: A systems neuroscience analysis.
Annu Rev Neurosci
2011
;
34
:
601
28
11.
Forman
SA
:
Molecular approaches to improved general anesthetics.
Anesthesiol Clin North America
2010
;
28
:
761
71
12.
Mashour
GA
,
Orser
BA
,
Avidan
MS
:
Intraoperative awareness: From neurobiology to clinical practice.
Anesthesiology
2011
;
114
:
1218
33