Background

Loss of consciousness is an essential feature of general anesthesia. Although alterations of neural networks during anesthesia have been identified in the spatial domain, there has been relatively little study of temporal organization.

Methods

Ten healthy male volunteers were anesthetized with an induction dose of propofol on two separate occasions. The duration of network connections in the brain was analyzed by multichannel electroencephalography and the minimum spanning tree method. Entropy of the connections was calculated based on Shannon entropy. The global temporal configuration of networks was investigated by constructing the cumulative distribution function of connection times in different frequency bands and different states of consciousness.

Results

General anesthesia was associated with a significant reduction in the number of network connections, as well as significant alterations of their duration. These changes were most prominent in the δ bandwidth and were also associated with a significant reduction in entropy of the connection matrix. Despite these and other changes, a global "scale-free" organization was consistently preserved across multiple subjects, anesthetic exposures, states of consciousness, and electroencephalogram frequencies.

Conclusions

Our data suggest a fundamental principle of temporal organization of network connectivity that is maintained during consciousness and anesthesia, despite local changes. These findings are consistent with a process of adaptive reconfiguration during general anesthesia.

  • ❖ Loss of consciousness is an essential feature of general anesthesia.

  • ❖ Altered network connections in the brain may contribute to loss of consciousness.

  • ❖ After induction with propofol, brain networks are altered but maintain an important feature of organization found during consciousness. Therefore, general anesthesia does not seem to be mediated by a complete network failure.

IN the early 18th century, mathematician Leonhard Euler solved the problem of the seven bridges of Königsberg, a Prussian city that spanned both sides of the Pregel river. The landmass of Königsberg, as well as several large islands, was connected by seven bridges—the puzzle consisted in finding a way to walk through the city while crossing each bridge only once. Euler demonstrated mathematically that no such path existed; by doing so, he put forth the first theorem in what is now known as graph theory . A graph  could be used to solve this problem, because each landmass could be represented as a node , or vertex , and each bridge connecting two nodes could be represented as an edge , or arc  (table 1).

Table 1.  Graph Theory Glossary

Table 1.  Graph Theory Glossary
Table 1.  Graph Theory Glossary

Graph theory is now used to represent connectivity in the brain, where neural regions are considered “nodes” and the relationships between them, the “edges.”1,2The graphs used to represent functional networks in the brain have a particular organization that is thought to be “scale free.” Scale-free organization can be identified mathematically when a system follows a “power law”versus  an exponential or Gaussian behavior (table 1). Unlike a random network, scale-free networks following a power law have a number of important hubs that are more densely connected, as in airport systems. This scale-free property has been widely observed in both the structure and function of the human brain and is thought to enable rapid synchronization and information transfer, minimal wiring costs, and a balance between local segregation and global integration.1–8 

General anesthesia induces a profound alteration of consciousness and is associated with changes in local and global networks in the brain.9,10Altered functional connectivity during anesthesia has been demonstrated within neural networks involving the thalamocortical and corticocortical systems11,12and may be dose-dependent.13A recent functional magnetic resonance imaging study reported that brain activities related to primary sensory areas and the default mode network were not significantly different after exposure to low concentrations of sevoflurane anesthesia although higher order cognitive regions were significantly altered.14It was suggested that functional brain organization is preserved under low levels of anesthesia, which is consistent with previous studies demonstrating a preserved default mode network even under deep anesthesia in monkeys.15 

To investigate the activity of neural networks during general anesthesia, we applied a technique derived from graph theory that uses objective criteria to reconstruct electroencephalographic channel connections based on the minimum spanning tree (MST) method. Because general anesthesia is associated with altered functional connectivity and loss of consciousness, we hypothesized that the optimal scale-free organization of human brain networks would be disrupted after induction with propofol.

Contrary to our hypothesis, we found that—despite significant local network changes—a global scale-free organization was preserved across consciousness, anesthesia, and recovery. These findings are consistent with “adaptive reconfiguration,” a previously identified process1in which the brain maintains a globally optimal state (i.e ., scale-free organization) despite local changes. The implications of these findings for the mechanisms of anesthetic-induced unconsciousness are discussed.

Methodological Overview

After institutional review board approval (Asan Medical Center, Seoul, Korea) and informed consent, 10 healthy male participants were studied on two separate occasions with 21-channel electroencephalography. Three states were investigated: (1) baseline consciousness; (2) general anesthesia, defined as loss of response to a command after induction with 2 mg/kg intravenous propofol; and (3) recovery from general anesthesia, defined as a return of responsiveness. These electroencephalographic data were originally gathered for a study of the frontoparietal system10but underwent a completely different analysis for the current study.

Because previous work focused on spatial network attributes, the focus of this investigation was temporal organization. To study the temporal evolution of network connectivity among electroencephalographic channels, the MST and moving window method were used.16,17The MST is the skeleton of a complex network that reflects the primary edge connections of the original network.18–20We constructed a sequence of MSTs from the moving electroencephalographic windows and examined the temporal evolution of edge connections during different states of consciousness. The conventional method of defining a connection, which requires a subjective threshold for connection strength, is not appropriate for general anesthesia because of the significant change of average electroencephalographic coherence. If a threshold is set at the baseline state, the construction of an edge connection will not be optimal in the unconscious state, showing only a few small islands or nothing at all on a connection matrix. This principle is demonstrated in figure 1A. The white patterns in figure 1Adenote the connected channels, for which Pearson correlation coefficients are above the threshold; the black patterns represent the unconnected channels below the threshold.

Fig. 1.  Analysis of networks using a conventional versus the minimum spanning tree method. (A ) This demonstrates the conventional method of using a threshold for connections, resulting in some connection matrices with only small islands (denoted with white) or no connections at all because of significantly different mean correlation coefficients across states. As an example, the first 10 connection matrices of the α band electroencephalogram for three states are presented (each box represents a 7-s nonoverlapping window). The connection matrices are constructed with a zero-lag Pearson correlation coefficient and a threshold. (B ) In this example, functional connections were constructed by the minimum spanning tree method in a 7-s nonoverlapping window. The connections on the minimum spanning tree are denoted with gray colored patterns on the 21 × 21 connection matrix with elements 0 ≤dij ≤√2 (F: Fp1, Fp2, F3, F4, F5, F6, F7, F8, Fz, C: C3, C4, Cz, T: T7, T8, P: P3, P4, P5, P6, P7, P8, Pz). (C ) Each type of electroencephalographic channel connection and its connectivity (denoted as 1 for connected and 0 for disconnected) is represented over time on a newly defined connection matrix. The matrix represents the connectivity among electroencephalographic channels over the entire recording period. Each row on the matrix corresponds to the electroencephalographic channel connection for each minimum spanning tree. S  (t ) denotes the time a connection lasts, and R  (t ) denotes the time required to re-establish a connection once it has been terminated. EEG = electroencephalography; LOC = loss of consciousness; ROC = return of consciousness.

Fig. 1.  Analysis of networks using a conventional versus the minimum spanning tree method. (A ) This demonstrates the conventional method of using a threshold for connections, resulting in some connection matrices with only small islands (denoted with white) or no connections at all because of significantly different mean correlation coefficients across states. As an example, the first 10 connection matrices of the α band electroencephalogram for three states are presented (each box represents a 7-s nonoverlapping window). The connection matrices are constructed with a zero-lag Pearson correlation coefficient and a threshold. (B ) In this example, functional connections were constructed by the minimum spanning tree method in a 7-s nonoverlapping window. The connections on the minimum spanning tree are denoted with gray colored patterns on the 21 × 21 connection matrix with elements 0 ≤dij ≤√2 (F: Fp1, Fp2, F3, F4, F5, F6, F7, F8, Fz, C: C3, C4, Cz, T: T7, T8, P: P3, P4, P5, P6, P7, P8, Pz). (C ) Each type of electroencephalographic channel connection and its connectivity (denoted as 1 for connected and 0 for disconnected) is represented over time on a newly defined connection matrix. The matrix represents the connectivity among electroencephalographic channels over the entire recording period. Each row on the matrix corresponds to the electroencephalographic channel connection for each minimum spanning tree. S  (t ) denotes the time a connection lasts, and R  (t ) denotes the time required to re-establish a connection once it has been terminated. EEG = electroencephalography; LOC = loss of consciousness; ROC = return of consciousness.

Close modal

To investigate connection time, we segmented the raw 21-channel electroencephalography into 7-s windows moving at 40 ms and then applied the MST method to assess network relationships using Pearson correlation. The output for each window is a set of primary network connections on the MST that can be shown as connected or disconnected over time. Figure 1Bdemonstrates an example of MST sequences, which are constructed with α band electroencephalographic data for three states. The gray patterns denote the connected channels that have distance values, whereas the black patterns denote unconnected channels. By assigning a “1” to maintained connections and “0” to terminated connections, a connection matrix can be generated with sequential MSTs. Figure 1Cis a schematic depicting the generation of such a connection matrix, with two temporal variables of interest: (1) the survival time (S  (t )) or duration of an edge connection, and (2) the reconnection time (R  (t )) or the time required to reestablish a connection once terminated. Because we ultimately found a reciprocal relationship between connection and reconnection times, only connection time data are presented. We constructed a connection matrix and calculated “connection entropy,” which is based on the concept of Shannon entropy and quantifies the complexity of the temporal configuration of network connections. This model enables the direct comparison of temporal configurations of brain functions for different conscious states.

To investigate the overall temporal organization of network connections, the cumulative density function of connection times was examined for different states and all frequency bands. The maximum likelihood method and Kolmogorov-Smirnov (KS) statistics were used to fit the cumulative density function of connection times. To find the best fit for connection time data, we used the KS statistics and the root-mean-square error (RMSE) method for the alternative models (i.e ., power law, stretched exponential, log-normal, and exponential distribution models). Power law models reflect a scale-free organization.

Drug Administration and Electroencephalography

Each volunteer fasted for 8 h before study drug administration. We excluded volunteers who had a known allergy to propofol, medical problems, abnormal laboratory findings with clinical significance, or a body weight that was not within 30% of ideal. Average volunteer age was 23 ± 2 yr (range, 20–28 yr).

An 18-gauge angiocatheter was placed in a vein of the antecubital area. Subjects received an initial intravenous bolus dose of 2.0 mg/kg, propofol, over 20 s. Time to loss of consciousness was determined every 5 s by the loss of response to verbal command. If respiratory depression occurred, lungs were manually ventilated with 100% oxygen via  facemask to maintain an end-tidal carbon dioxide concentration of 35–45 mmHg. Manual ventilation was discontinued when the spontaneous respiratory rate exceeded 12 breaths/min and end-tidal carbon dioxide was less than 45 mmHg. Time to return of consciousness after the intravenous bolus of 2.0 mg/kg, propofol, was determined by the recovery of response to verbal command. Upon completion of the measurement of electroencephalographic activity, subjects were transferred to the postanesthesia care unit, breathing room air. They were monitored with electrocardiography, pulse oximetry, and noninvasive blood pressure measurement for 1 h in recovery. If vital signs were stable and full recovery from sedation was confirmed, the subject was discharged. Each subject carried out the same experimental protocol separated by a 7-day washout period.

The electroencephalograms of 21 channels (Fp1, Fp2, F3, F4, F5, F6, F7, F8, Fz, C3, C4, Cz, T7, T8, P3, P4, P5, P6, P7, P8, Pz referenced by A2, 10–20 system) were recorded on the bed with closed eyes, with a sampling frequency of 256 Hz and 16-bit analog-to-digital precision by WEEG-32® (LXE3232-RF; Laxtha Inc., Daejeon, Korea). Baseline electroencephalography was recorded for 5 min before an intravenous bolus dose of propofol. Electroencephalography was recorded continuously during and after bolus dosing, and up to 10 min after return of consciousness. The same procedure was repeated for the same 10 healthy male subjects 1 week later.

Minimum Spanning Tree

MST is one way to define global network relationships among channels for electroencephalographic data. The MST is defined as the connected, loopless subgraph consisting of (N − 1) links reaching all N  nodes while minimizing the sum of the relative edge weights. For satisfying the minimum weight condition in the linkage of N  channels, each electroencephalographic channel is linked to the most closely correlated channel without making a loop in the reconstructed network. In this study, therefore, the edge connection and disconnection for a pair of channels was defined as whether the “closest relationship” on the MST was maintained or not; we limited network reconstruction to the 20 most closely correlated channels, reflecting the principal network structure. The MST structure also shares similar topological properties with the original network, such as “betweenness” centralities, clustering coefficient, and degree distribution.18–20 

First, we defined a distance matrix based on the Pearson correlation coefficients, −1 ≤Cij ≤ 1, among 21 electroencephalographic channels (i , j = 1, …, N . N = 21). Zero-lag correlations were studied because they enabled a nonparametric analytic technique. Next, the N ×N  matrix of correlation coefficients was transformed into an N ×N  distance matrix with elements

, such that 0 ≤dij ≤√2, which were introduced by Mantegna.21By this definition, the elements dij  satisfy the axioms for the metric distances, as well as those for ultrametric distances (a stronger triangular inequality), producing a tree-like hierarchical connection structure among all nodes. For the metric distance, the difference of negative or positive correlation coefficient was ignored. The MST is constructed by connecting all the N  channels with N − 1 connections such that the sum of all connections, Σ(i,j )dij , is a minimum. If we assume that no electroencephalographic channels are the same, it enables us to define the unique network connections for a given multichannel electroencephalographic data set by the minimum condition. This method is advantageous because it avoids subjective parameter decisions in the conventional way, reflects the original network with small number of connections (N − 1) instead of (N ×N ), and maintains topological attributes. Therefore, we were able to construct the sequence of edge connections corresponding to all electroencephalographic windows, irrespective of significant change of average electroencephalographic coherence during general anesthesia.

Connection Entropy

To quantify the complexity of a connection matrix, we used a measure based on the definition of Shannon entropy. This process quantifies the temporal complexity of network connectivity for multichannel electroencephalograms and enabled us to quantitatively compare the temporal configurations of brain functions for different conscious states.

For a connection matrix, each type of electroencephalographic connection derived from the MST (i.e ., each connection matrix column) is considered as an event among all possible events (N ), consisting of edge connections occurring for a given electroencephalogram. The frequency of an event is counted by the number “1” in the corresponding column of the connection matrix. The probability of an event (e.g ., an edge connection between channel i  and j ) is defined as pij =nij /M , where nij  is the frequency of an event (i,j ) and M  is the total number of samples (i.e ., the total number of segmented windows, M = 7,325 for a 5-min epoch with a 7-s window size moving every 40 ms). The connection entropy is defined by the Shannon entropy, H =−ΣN(ij)=1pij  log2pij .

If the chance of an edge connection is equiprobable over all pairs of electroencephalographic channels, the connection entropy has a higher value. On the other hand, if the connections are dominated by a few pairs of electroencephalographic channels, the connection entropy has a smaller value. This procedure was applied to the electroencephalographic data after Fourier-based band-pass filtering of the following frequency bands in the three different states of consciousness: δ (0.5–4 Hz), θ (4–8 Hz), α (8–13 Hz), β (13–35 Hz), and original (0.5–55 Hz). High-frequency (γ) activity (i.e ., more than 35 Hz) was not analyzed separately to avoid electromyographic artifact.

Power Law Test

A statistical test of power-law distribution that would reflect scale-free organization was performed. First, we used the maximum likelihood method to fit the cumulative density function of connection times. Second, we used the KS statistic to estimate where the scaling region begins (>X  min). This method gives the empirically best scaling region by optimizing the KS goodness of fit statistic. Third, we tested the goodness of fit by the KS statistic, in which the null hypothesis is that the empirical data follow the hypothesized power law distribution. The KS statistic calculates the maximum discrepancy between the empirical distribution and the theoretical one. Here, if there is statistical significance (i.e ., P > 0.01), the null hypothesis of a power law model cannot be rejected with the significance level of 99%. Finally, we compared the alternative models (i.e ., power law, stretched exponential, log-normal, and exponential distribution models) for the best fit to the connection times data, with the following procedure: (1) for fitting a given data set with the power-law model, we performed the first and the second processes described to get the empirically best scaling region in the data; (2) RMSE was calculated over the optimal scaling region; (3) we applied the stretched exponential, log-normal, and exponential models, respectively, for fitting the same data; and (4) we compared the RMSE of the all fitting models within the optimal scaling region (>X  min). We used the Matlab and R codes released for the first to the third process above; for the RMSE calculation of all fitting models, the Matlab statistics toolbox (The MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA) was used.

Random Connections

To compare the network connectivity of electroencephalograms with a random process, we generated a random electroencephalographic data set. The values of each channel were randomly shuffled by a Fourier-based phase-randomized method,22eliminating the original correlation between and within electroencephalographic channels. Next, a connection matrix of randomized electroencephalographs was constructed in the same way as described above for the actual electroencephalographic data. The connection duration was calculated for the connection matrices generated from randomized electroencephalograms. The distributions of connection times for the randomized data were compared with those of original electroencephalographs in different states.

Statistical Analysis

In addition to the statistical tests used to analyze distribution patterns, the connection times and the connection entropy were compared across three states of consciousness (i.e ., baseline, unconscious, recovery) with 20 electroencephalographic data sets (i.e ., 10 subjects undergoing two trials each). The significance was assessed by a repeated measures one-way analysis of variance with post hoc  analysis using Scheffé's multicomparison. A P  value lower than 0.05 was considered significant. The post hoc  Scheffé's test is considered more conservative then the post hoc  Tukey test. A formal statistical consultation was obtained at the University of Michigan and the Matlab statistical toolbox was used. Data are expressed as mean ± SD.

Local Network Connections and Connection Times

Using the moving window method and the MST method, we constructed connection matrices for temporal analysis of edge connections during different states of consciousness. After induction of general anesthesia, the duration of local edge connections was significantly changed in the transition of states across different frequency bands (P < 0.05, Scheffé's post hoc  test). For each pair of electroencephalographic channels, the 20 connection times defined by the MST were compared across three states. If the connection time was significantly different from the mean value, the edge between channels was denoted with dotted and solid lines on a 2-dimensional scalp map (fig. 2). Dotted lines denoting significantly reduced connection times and solid lines denoting significantly increased connection times are presented in figure 2across different frequency bands (P < 0.05, Scheffé's post hoc  test). Although there were numerous changes, the δ band demonstrated the most significant alterations of local connections after loss and return of consciousness.

Fig. 2.  Significant changes of the connection time at local functional connections. The solid lines  and dotted lines  indicate significant differences of connection time comparing the three states for each pair of electroencephalographic channels (P < 0.05, Scheffé's post hoc  test). The solid line  in each figure means that LOC > Baseline for the comparison of LOC & Baseline, ROC > LOC for the comparison of LOC & ROC, and ROC > Baseline for the comparison of ROC & Baseline. The dotted line  indicates the reverse. The channel names are positioned by the international 10-20 system of electrode placement. EEG = electroencephalogram; LOC = loss of consciousness; ROC = return of consciousness.

Fig. 2.  Significant changes of the connection time at local functional connections. The solid lines  and dotted lines  indicate significant differences of connection time comparing the three states for each pair of electroencephalographic channels (P < 0.05, Scheffé's post hoc  test). The solid line  in each figure means that LOC > Baseline for the comparison of LOC & Baseline, ROC > LOC for the comparison of LOC & ROC, and ROC > Baseline for the comparison of ROC & Baseline. The dotted line  indicates the reverse. The channel names are positioned by the international 10-20 system of electrode placement. EEG = electroencephalogram; LOC = loss of consciousness; ROC = return of consciousness.

Close modal

Entropy of the Connection Matrix

Changes in duration of local connections in the different bandwidths during anesthesia were paralleled by changes in the overall entropy of the connection matrices. Figure 3demonstrates connection matrices for the δ band of the electroencephalogram during consciousness (fig. 3A), anesthesia (fig. 3B), and recovery (fig. 3C). We focused on the δ band because of the numerous local changes demonstrated in figure 2. The connection entropy of the δ and θ bands was significantly decreased after induction of general anesthesia and returned again after recovery of consciousness (P < 0.05, Scheffé's post hoc  test) (fig. 3D). The connection entropy of the original electroencephalogram (0.5–55 Hz) was also significantly reduced after loss of consciousness (P < 0.05, Scheffé's post hoc  test). The connection entropies of all frequency bands were completely discriminated from that of randomized electroencephalograms (mean ± SD = 65.27 ± 0.2 bits), with significantly lower entropies than that of randomized electroencephalographic data.

Fig. 3.  The generation of connection matrices and alterations of connection entropy. Examples of connection matrices, constructed with the electroencephalograms of the delta band, are presented for the baseline conscious state (A ), unconscious state (B ), and recovery state (C ). For a 5-min long electroencephalogram, we can derive 7,325 minimum spanning trees from 7,325 segmented moving windows. In theory, we could get 210 possible edges, but because of limitations relating to the timeframe as well as the neural architecture itself, some edges are not formed. In A –C , approximately 150 edges appeared for baseline conscious state, approximately 60 edges for loss of consciousness, and approximately 100 edges for return of consciousness. The number of total edges is different depending on the subject and state. These matrices were generated using the method described in figure 1C. For each “1” representing a connection, a white dot is placed. Each row represents a minimum spanning tree identifying the channels connected—thus, of all of the connections observed, only the top 20 derived from the method are shown on each row. The decrease in heterogeneity of connected channels and increase in the duration of the retained channels can be seen after loss of consciousness. The exact types of connection edges (i.e ., electroencephalography channel connections) are not matched on the x  axes. (D ) The mean connection entropies of the five frequency bands in the three states are presented (the error bar denotes standard deviation; **P < 0.01, *P < 0.05, Scheffé's post hoc  test). EEG = electroencephalogram; LOC = loss of consciousness; ROC = return of consciousness.

Fig. 3.  The generation of connection matrices and alterations of connection entropy. Examples of connection matrices, constructed with the electroencephalograms of the delta band, are presented for the baseline conscious state (A ), unconscious state (B ), and recovery state (C ). For a 5-min long electroencephalogram, we can derive 7,325 minimum spanning trees from 7,325 segmented moving windows. In theory, we could get 210 possible edges, but because of limitations relating to the timeframe as well as the neural architecture itself, some edges are not formed. In A –C , approximately 150 edges appeared for baseline conscious state, approximately 60 edges for loss of consciousness, and approximately 100 edges for return of consciousness. The number of total edges is different depending on the subject and state. These matrices were generated using the method described in figure 1C. For each “1” representing a connection, a white dot is placed. Each row represents a minimum spanning tree identifying the channels connected—thus, of all of the connections observed, only the top 20 derived from the method are shown on each row. The decrease in heterogeneity of connected channels and increase in the duration of the retained channels can be seen after loss of consciousness. The exact types of connection edges (i.e ., electroencephalography channel connections) are not matched on the x  axes. (D ) The mean connection entropies of the five frequency bands in the three states are presented (the error bar denotes standard deviation; **P < 0.01, *P < 0.05, Scheffé's post hoc  test). EEG = electroencephalogram; LOC = loss of consciousness; ROC = return of consciousness.

Close modal

Scale-Free, Power-Law Distribution

Despite statistically significant differences in the connection times and connection entropies associated with consciousness, anesthesia, and recovery, the distribution of connection times primarily followed a power-law distribution in all three states, which is indicative of preserved scale-free organization (fig. 4A). The exponents of the distribution, as well as their relationship to window size, are demonstrated in figure 4, B and C. In practice, few empirical phenomena obey power laws for all values. Instead, the power law applies only for values greater than some minimum value (X  min).23In our data, the power distribution appeared on the tail of the distribution of connection times (i.e ., more than 10 s), although exogenous and endogenous random effects of empirical data were reflected on smaller duration range (i.e ., less than 10s), despite clear discrimination from the randomized electroencephalographic data (fig. 4A).

Fig. 4.  Scale-free distribution of connection times across multiple states of consciousness. (A ) The cumulative distribution function for the pooled survival times of all subjects (and each subject) in the three states. The cumulative distribution function of the pooled connection times of the original electroencephalogram shows straight lines on the log-log scale graph, as well as for an individual subject (s1). The cumulative distribution function of the connection times of randomized electroencephalographic data shows exponential (i.e., Gaussian) distributions, which is clearly distinguishable from that of the connection time data of the original electroencephalograph. Blue dot = baseline conscious state; red dot = unconscious state; green dot = recovery state; black dots = three randomized electroencephalographs; black dashed line = best scale region determined by Kolmogorov-Smirnov statistic. In the inset, the red  and blue lines  denote the three states for the first and second (1 week later) data sets, respectively, demonstrating the reproducibility of the finding. (B ) The mean scaling exponents fitted from cumulative distribution function for all data sets in the window size 1,800 (∼7 s). The error bar  denotes SD. The scaling exponents across the three states are indistinguishable across all frequency bands, except for baseline delta. (C ) The mean scaling exponents of the original electroencephalogram for three states over the different window sizes. This demonstrates that the scaling exponents converge around the window size 500 (∼2 s). (D ) The root-mean square error (RMSE) of the alternative fitting models for the connection time data in the frequency bands in the three states are presented in double logarithm scales. The RMSE of the power law is consistently lower than those of the other models in all cases. circle = baseline; square = loss of consciousness (LOC); triangle = return of consciousness (ROC).

Fig. 4.  Scale-free distribution of connection times across multiple states of consciousness. (A ) The cumulative distribution function for the pooled survival times of all subjects (and each subject) in the three states. The cumulative distribution function of the pooled connection times of the original electroencephalogram shows straight lines on the log-log scale graph, as well as for an individual subject (s1). The cumulative distribution function of the connection times of randomized electroencephalographic data shows exponential (i.e., Gaussian) distributions, which is clearly distinguishable from that of the connection time data of the original electroencephalograph. Blue dot = baseline conscious state; red dot = unconscious state; green dot = recovery state; black dots = three randomized electroencephalographs; black dashed line = best scale region determined by Kolmogorov-Smirnov statistic. In the inset, the red  and blue lines  denote the three states for the first and second (1 week later) data sets, respectively, demonstrating the reproducibility of the finding. (B ) The mean scaling exponents fitted from cumulative distribution function for all data sets in the window size 1,800 (∼7 s). The error bar  denotes SD. The scaling exponents across the three states are indistinguishable across all frequency bands, except for baseline delta. (C ) The mean scaling exponents of the original electroencephalogram for three states over the different window sizes. This demonstrates that the scaling exponents converge around the window size 500 (∼2 s). (D ) The root-mean square error (RMSE) of the alternative fitting models for the connection time data in the frequency bands in the three states are presented in double logarithm scales. The RMSE of the power law is consistently lower than those of the other models in all cases. circle = baseline; square = loss of consciousness (LOC); triangle = return of consciousness (ROC).

Close modal

The slope of the linear portion of the distribution is indistinguishable across all subjects, all states, and all electroencephalographic bands except the baseline δ. More than 80% of connection duration data of all subjects in all frequencies and states passed the KS test, which has the null hypothesis for a power-law model. The goodness of fit for the power law model by the KS statistic showed that 82.4% of the connection duration data of 20 electroencephalographic data sets for all frequency bands and all states have a P  value higher than 0.05 in the case of window size 1,800 (76.6%, baseline conscious state; 87.0%, unconscious state; 80.5%, recovery state).

In terms of test-test reproducibility, the two data sets at 1-week time intervals demonstrated the same power law distributions for all frequency bands (see fig. 4A, inset). Comparing the KS statistics across the two anesthetic exposures, 82.1% and 82.7% of the first and second data sets, respectively, passed the null hypothesis with P  values higher than 0.05. For the dependency of P  values on window size, the hypothesis of the power law model cannot be rejected for most of the connection duration data beyond a certain window size (i.e ., more than 1,000 or ∼4 s).

The comparison with alternative models (power law, stretched exponential, log-normal, and exponential distributions) demonstrates that the power law is the best-fitting model for the connection time data for all frequency bands in all states (fig. 4D). The limited length of the electroencephalographic data and relatively small number of electroencephalographic channels may reduce the possibility of rare events of connection times, which is one of the distinctive characteristics of a power law distribution compared with stretched exponential and log-normal distributions. Nonetheless, the comparison of the RMSE consistently indicates the power law as the best fitting model for the probability distribution of connection durations over all frequency bands (fig. 4D).

With the use of a method based on graph theory, we have identified changes in the temporal organization of brain networks across consciousness, anesthesia, and recovery. More importantly, however, there is the strong indication of a preserved scale-free or power-law organization that persists across multiple subjects, multiple anesthetic exposures, multiple states of consciousness, and multiple electroencephalographic bandwidths. Thus, despite the fact that consciousness is suppressed during general anesthesia and altered during recovery—both reflected by significant changes in the connection times and entropy—a fundamental principle of temporal organization seems to be maintained. Maintaining a global scale-free organization in the face of local alterations is suggestive of a process referred to as “adaptive reconfiguration.”1 

Temporal Organization of Networks

In terms of local organization, we observed that the connection entropy of the lower frequency bands (δ, θ) were significantly reduced after loss of consciousness and restored after return of consciousness. These results suggest that the network connectivity of the lower frequency bands becomes less random during anesthesia, which is consistent with past findings.24In terms of global organization, numerous studies have reported scale-free and self-similar properties for other temporal variables in the brain, including the amplitude fluctuation of electroencephalographic oscillations,3the global synchronization of electroencephalographic signals,25and the power-spectral density.7Using two methods of neuroimaging, a recent study in healthy volunteers demonstrated that phase-locked intervals for two signals were in a power-law distribution.26These studies strongly suggest that the power law is a plausible model for the temporal configuration of networks in the brain. Our data support this by showing persistent scale-free organization of connection durations across multiple subjects, multiple anesthetic exposures, multiple states, and multiple electroencephalographic bandwidths.

The inability to assert with complete certainty that the distribution pattern observed is based on a power law is mirrored in similar controversies regarding distribution patterns of connectivity in the spatial domain.4,5,27,28What is critical to note is that—independent of the precise identity of distribution of these temporal parameters—figure 4Cdemonstrates clearly that the scaling exponents are preserved across states as the window size increases. The consistent organization in the temporal domain during consciousness, general anesthesia, and recovery is an unexpected finding, especially given the numerous local perturbations, as well as the dramatic change of the behavioral state.

Implications for Anesthetic Mechanisms

Recent data in animal29and human9,10,30models have validated frameworks describing the deintegration31,32or unbinding33–35of cortical processing as a mechanism of anesthetic-induced consciousness. The current data suggest, however, that although higher order cortical integration may be disrupted during general anesthesia, certain dynamic principles of the underlying network are maintained. Thus, the state of general anesthesia does not seem to be a complete network failure in which a breakdown of scale-free organization occurs. Indeed, our findings are consistent with data in humans demonstrating that neural networks are characterized by scale-free organization in the spatial domain1,2,4–6but the brain undergoes an “adaptive reconfiguration” preserving global topological parameters despite a change in state associated with task performance.1,36Because the scale-free properties of the connection times were maintained in our study, a similar process may occur under general anesthesia. Our data therefore point to a mechanism that maintains the optimal brain state by the reconfiguration of neural networks, despite significant alteration of local network connections as a result of the effects of anesthesia.

To clarify the precise relationship of anesthetic mechanisms and network organization, further work is required. First, it is important to note that the MST method reflects the principal network structure, not the entire network. Network properties may vary with different definitions of connectivity and different modeling techniques. For example, including nonphase-lagged connections that reflect long-range communication in the brain could yield different results.

Second, a more precise understanding of differential network effects is required. For example, it is clear that certain network structures, such as the default mode network, are preserved even under anesthesia.15It is possible that other networks mediating conscious processing are nonetheless selectively disrupted and may lose scale-free organization.

Third, it is important to note that we excluded higher frequency γ activity because of the potential confound of electromyographic activity, although findings in the γ band were consistent with those of other frequencies (data not shown). It has previously been demonstrated by John et al .37that loss of γ-band coherence was associated with the loss of consciousness induced by multiple agents. We have also shown a selective breakdown of γ activity after anesthetic-induced unconsciousness.9Future studies should include the analysis of higher frequency activity that controls for electromyography artifact.

Finally, the combined analysis of neurophysiological data with anatomical data, such as electroencephalography during functional magnetic resonance imaging,38may yield further insights into the temporal and spatial network changes during general anesthesia. Ultimately, these data will need to be integrated with the known molecular actions of general anesthetics.

Limitations

Our study has numerous limitations. First, a relatively small number of young, healthy men were studied. Further work with female subjects, older subjects, and those with comorbidities will be necessary to establish this finding. Second, a bolus dose rather than a steady-state concentration of propofol was studied—a practice that models the routine clinical event of anesthetic induction. The preservation of scale-free organization after an induction dose of propofol is perhaps more compelling and unexpected than after a small change in steady-state concentration, as a bolus dose is likely far more disruptive to neural processing. Third, we studied only one intravenous agent. Future studies should include other intravenous agents as well as inhalational anesthetics. Finally, disadvantages of both the MST method and electroencephalography in general are suboptimal spatial resolution and potentially confounding effects from unexpected artifacts and common source input.

The induction of general anesthesia with propofol is associated with numerous local changes in the temporal organization of network connections, most notably in the δ bandwidth. Despite these and other changes in connection entropy, a global scale-free organization is preserved across multiple subjects, anesthetic exposures, states of consciousness, and electroencephalogram bandwidths. This study highlights the utility of graph theory in the study of anesthetic mechanisms and introduces the possibility of adaptive reconfiguration as a neural property of general anesthesia.

1.
Bassett DS, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Achard S, Duke T, Bullmore E: Adaptive reconfiguration of fractal small-world human brain functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2006; 103:19518–23
2.
Bullmore E, Sporns O: Complex brain networks: Graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nat Rev Neurosci 2009; 10:186–98
3.
Linkenkaer-Hansen K, Nikouline VV, Palva JM, Ilmoniemi RJ: Long-range temporal correlations and scaling behavior in human brain oscillations. J Neurosci 2001; 21:1370–7
4.
van den Heuvel MP, Stam CJ, Boersma M, Hulshoff Pol HE: Small-world and scale-free organization of voxel-based resting-state functional connectivity in the human brain. Neuroimage 2008; 43:528–39
5.
Eguíluz VM, Chialvo DR, Cecchi GA, Baliki M, Apkarian AV: Scale-free brain functional networks. Phys Rev Lett 2005; 94:018102–7
6.
Sporns O, Chialvo DR, Kaiser M, Hilgetag CC: Organization, development and function of complex brain networks. Trends Cogn Sci 2004; 8:418–25
7.
Bédard C, Kröger H, Destexhe A: Does the 1/f frequency scaling of brain signals reflect self-organized critical states? Phys Rev Lett 2006; 97:118102–5
8.
Sporns O, Tononi G, Kötter R: The human connectome: A structural description of the human brain. PLoS Comput Biol 2005; 1:e42
9.
Lee U, Mashour GA, Kim S, Noh GJ, Choi BM: Propofol induction reduces the capacity for neural information integration: Implications for the mechanism of consciousness and general anesthesia. Conscious Cogn 2009; 18:56–64
10.
Lee U, Kim S, Noh GJ, Choi BM, Hwang E, Mashour GA: The directionality and functional organization of frontoparietal connectivity during consciousness and anesthesia in humans. Conscious Cogn 2009; 18:1069–78
11.
White NS, Alkire MT: Impaired thalamocortical connectivity in humans during general-anesthetic-induced unconsciousness. Neuroimage 2003; 19:402–11
12.
Alkire MT: Loss of effective connectivity during general anesthesia. Int Anesthesiol Clin 2008; 46:55–73
13.
Peltier SJ, Kerssens C, Hamann SB, Sebel PS, Byas-Smith M, Hu X: Functional connectivity changes with concentration of sevoflurane anesthesia. Neuroreport 2005; 16:285–8
14.
Martuzzi R, Ramani R, Qiu M, Rajeevan N, Constable RT: Functional connectivity and alterations in baseline brain state in humans. Neuroimage 2010; 49:823–34
15.
Vincent JL, Patel GH, Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Baker JT, Van Essen DC, Zempel JM, Snyder LH, Corbetta M, Raichle ME: Intrinsic functional architecture in the anaesthetized monkey brain. Nature 2007; 447:83–6
16.
Everitt BS: Cluster Analysis. Edited by Heinemann Educational for the Social Science Research Council. London, Halsted Press, 1974Heinemann Educational for the Social Science Research Council
London
,
Halsted Press
17.
Lee U, Kim S, Jung KY: Classification of epilepsy types through global network analysis of scalp electroencephalograms. Phys Rev E Stat Nonlin Soft Matter Phys 2006; 73:041920–9
18.
Kim DH, Noh JD, Jeong H: Scale-free trees: The skeletons of complex networks. Phys Rev E Stat Nonlin Soft Matter Phys 2004; 70:046126–31
19.
Goh KI, Salvi G, Kahng B, Kim D: Skeleton and fractal scaling in complex networks. Phys Rev Lett 2006; 96:018701
20.
Braunstein LA, Wu Z, Chen Y, Buldyrev SV, Sreenivasan S, Kalisky T, Cohen R, Lopez E, Havlin S, Stanley HE: Optimal path and minimal spanning trees in random weighted networks. Int J Bifurcat Chaos 2007; 17:2215–5
21.
Mantegna RN: Hierarchical structure in financial markets. Eur Phys J B 1999; 11:193–7
22.
Schreiber T, Schmitz A: Surrogate time series. Physica D 2000; 142:346–82
23.
Clauset A, Shalizi CR, Newman ME: Power-law distributions in empirical data. SIAM Rev 2009; 51:661–703
24.
Jordan D, Stockmanns G, Kochs EF, Pilge S, Schneider G: Electroencephalographic order pattern analysis for the separation of consciousness and unconsciousness: An analysis of approximate entropy, permutation entropy, recurrence rate, and phase coupling of order recurrence plots. Anesthesiology 2008; 109:1014–22
25.
Stam CJ, de Bruin EA: Scale-free dynamics of global functional connectivity in the human brain. Hum Brain Mapp 2004; 22:97–109
26.
Kitzbichler MG, Smith ML, Christensen SR, Bullmore E: Broadband criticality of human brain network synchronization. PLoS Comput Biol 2009; 5:e1000314
27.
He Y, Chen ZJ, Evans AC: Small-world anatomical networks in the human brain revealed by cortical thickness from MRI. Cereb Cortex 2007; 17:2407–19
28.
Gong G, He Y, Concha L, Lebel C, Gross DW, Evans AC, Beaulieu C: Mapping anatomical connectivity patterns of human cerebral cortex using in vivo  diffusion tensor imaging tractography. Cereb Cortex 2009; 19:524–36
29.
Hudetz AG, Vizuete JA, Imas OA: Desflurane selectively suppresses long-latency cortical neuronal response to flash in the rat. Anesthesiology 2009; 111:231–9
30.
Ferrarelli F, Massimini M, Sarasso S, Casali A, Riedner BA, Angelini G, Tononi G, Pearce RA: Breakdown in cortical effective connectivity during midazolam-induced loss of consciousness. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2010; 107:2681–6
31.
Hudetz AG: Suppressing consciousness: Mechanisms of general anesthesia. Semin Anesth 2006; 25:196–204
32.
Alkire MT, Hudetz AG, Tononi G: Consciousness and anesthesia. Science 2008; 322:876–80
33.
Mashour GA: Consciousness unbound: Toward a paradigm of general anesthesia. Anesthesiology 2004; 100:428–33
34.
John ER, Prichep LS: The anesthetic cascade: A theory of how anesthesia suppresses consciousness. Anesthesiology 2005; 102:447–71
35.
Mashour GA: Integrating the science of consciousness and anesthesia. Anesth Analg 2006; 103:975–82
36.
Sporns O, Honey CJ: Small worlds inside big brains. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2006; 103:19219–20
37.
John ER, Prichep LS, Kox W, Valdés-Sosa P, Bosch-Bayard J, Aubert E, Tom M, di Michele F, Gugino LD, diMichele F: Invariant reversible QEEG effects of anesthetics. Conscious Cogn 2001; 10:165–83
38.
Purdon PL, Pierce ET, Bonmassar G, Walsh J, Harrell PG, Kwo J, Deschler D, Barlow M, Merhar RC, Lamus C, Mullaly CM, Sullivan M, Maginnis S, Skoniecki D, Higgins HA, Brown EN: Simultaneous electroencephalography and functional magnetic resonance imaging of general anesthesia. Ann NY Acad Sci 2009; 1157:61–70