We are gratified by the interest generated by the publication of our recent article.1The accompanying letters raise important issues and questions relevant to our article and to the question of anesthetic neurotoxicity as it applies to children. The concerns expressed by the various authors can be categorized as follows: (1) The observed effect may reflect comorbidity or other unidentified factors rather than the effects of anesthesia per se  (Arul and Thies, Pysyk et al. , Taylor); (2) the definitions for learning disability (LD) were inappropriate (Pysyk et al. , Tolpin and Collard); (3) hypoxia or hyperoxia may be responsible for the observed effects (Coté, Mitchell, Kopp); and (4) the underlying relevant animal data are flawed (Taylor). As to number 4, we did not participate in the many animal studies, and given that the animal data have been recently reviewed by Loepke and others,2–7we will simply refer the reader to those studies, reviews, and editorials.

Clearly, we share the concern expressed by several of the authors for the need to control or adjust for comorbidity. However, we also recognize the difficulty of doing so in retrospective studies (or even in prospective studies) involving children. Arul and Thies suggest that comorbidity is the “elephant in the room.” We completely agree and extensively discussed this clear limitation of our data in our article. A cautionary sentence appears in the abstract, and this limitation is discussed at length in the body of the article, most clearly as follows: “These data cannot reveal whether exposure to anesthesia itself may contribute to the pathogenesis of LD, or whether the need for anesthesia is a marker for other unidentified confounding factors that contribute to LD.” We also chose not to include the positive findings in the article's title.

We appreciate similar concerns expressed by Pysyk et al.  regarding the difficulty of determining whether the effect observed in our study was the result of the surgical indication rather than the exposure to anesthesia per se . In our cohort, as would be true in any community-based sample, otolaryngologic procedures are the majority of the total, and children requiring myringotomy or tonsillectomy may indeed be predisposed to the adverse effects of sleep disturbance and/or hearing deficiency on learning. However, if surgical treatment of these conditions is efficacious and results in catch-up growth and development in those undergoing surgery, and if not all children receive surgical treatment, those not  undergoing the procedure may be at greatest risk for the neurocognitive and speech problems described by Pysyk et al. ,8,9which would bias against the observed effect of multiple surgeries on learning abilities. Also, the relation between this (and many other) condition(s) and the development of learning disabilities is not always clear. Arul and Thies cite a 1983 article that suggests that minor conditions such as otitis media are known to be associated with educational delay. The cited review of the existing literature of that time concluded that, “children who have been medically managed [with otitis media] have minimal deficits.” A subsequent article failed to demonstrate an increase in LD among children who were surgically managed for recurrent otitis media.10A recent Cochrane review suggests that it is uncertain that otitis media represents a risk for language or speech delay, and as a consequence, surgical treatment is of unclear benefit. Other studies have demonstrated that among children with language delay of unclear etiology, the only factors of significance were those controlled for in our analysis, e.g. , hearing abnormalities were not found to be predictive.11 

Ideally, extensive information regarding comorbid conditions would be available in a sample of children large enough to allow the subanalysis suggested by Pysyk et al.  Realistically, however, controlling for comorbidity is much more difficult than may be appreciated. Unfortunately, no uniformly recognized measure of burden of illness exists for children, requiring that we rely on measures such as the American Society of Anesthesiologists (ASA) physical status (PS) score. Arul and Thies point out that many of those children with multiple exposures had comorbid conditions that may predispose them to LD. LD was not, however, clustered among those with the greatest burden of illness as measured by the ASA PS. In fact, among the 144 children with multiple exposures, only 11% (2 of 19) with an ASA PS of greater than 2 had LD, whereas among those with an ASA PS of 2 or less, 34% (43 of 125) had LD. Therefore, it is by no means clear that the burden of comorbidity, as reflected by ASA PS, is associated with an increased risk of LD. Like Taylor, we also recognize the problems associated with the use of the ASA PS in this setting but also appreciate that no alternative measure is available. Similarly, we could not, as she suggests, control for comorbidity in the exposed group and not do so in the comparison group. To do so would have required that we individually abstract the complete medical records of more than 5,000 children. In an ongoing analysis using the same cohort, we, in partnership with the U.S. Food and Drug Administration, are in the process of examining, in a case–control design, the comorbid conditions of both cases and controls in an attempt to better control for both medical and surgical diagnosis. We hope that this will provide more insight into the concerns expressed.

The definitions used to determine LD in the birth cohort were those used for the original incidence (not prevalence) studies performed using the Rochester Epidemiology Project. Those studies used four methods to determine the incidence of various types of LD. For the study by Wilder et al. , one method (Shayvitz) was eliminated because it was deemed to be redundant. The rates quoted by Tolpin and Collard from our group's previous publications are for the incidence of the individual types of LD (math, reading, etc. ). The higher rate that we reported was because our outcome was the development of one or more types of LD. As described in the article, we chose this as an outcome because (1) we had no data to suggest that one type of LD (math, reading, etc. ) is more likely in this setting and (2) to examine a single type of LD would have dramatically reduced the statistical power of the study. For the same reason, we were not able to perform subanalyses to determine whether the observed effect was concentrated in one or more types of LD, but agree that this would be a fruitful topic for future investigations of sufficient power to conduct this analysis. In addition, LD as determined by the National Health Interview Survey and the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edition, Text Revision , are measures of prevalence rather than incidence and therefore cannot be directly compared with the incidence rates found in the Rochester Schools. Furthermore, the definition of prevalence found in the National Health Interview Survey is based on questionnaire responses to “Ever told sample child had a learning disability” rather than specific testing as was used in our cohort. The observation by Pysyk et al.  regarding our use of a cutoff of 1.75 SDs rather than the conventional 2 SDs is correct. This was chosen because it was the criterion in use by the state of Minnesota at that time.

Mitchell and Coté raise an issue that we did not discuss, positing unrecognized hypoxia as an explanation for the increase in LD observed in our cohort. The studies by Coté do not address the issue of cognitive impairment but do suggest that unrecognized hypoxia frequently occurred before the widespread adoption of pulse oximetry. Likewise, the presence of brief modest hypocapnia (a finding that occurred in only 9 of 260 total events and may have been as brief as 60 s in his study) is suggested as a potential confounder.12To our knowledge, brief hypocapnia has not been linked to subsequent deficits in learning, although among preterm neonates sustained profound hypocapnia has been suggested as a cause of periventricular leukomalacia, a pathology that is highly unlikely to have contributed to our findings. Interestingly, studies of hyperoxia in neonates have examined the effect of the prolonged oxygen saturations as low as 70% on the incidence and severity of retinopathy of prematurity. Those studies have failed to show an adverse neurocognitive effect in follow up as long as 18 months,13,14suggesting that even prolonged periods of hypoxia may be relatively well tolerated in children. Conversely, Kopp suggests that hyperoxia could lead to LD, observing that virtually all children in our cohort received a 30:70 mixture of oxygen and nitrous oxide. The degree of hyperoxia that could result from this mixture is modest. Furthermore, we are not aware of studies that link LD to oxygen exposure in young children, nor were studies cited that associate hyperoxia with abnormalities in memory, cognition, and learning in animals. The studies previously mentioned examining hyperoxia and its relation to retinopathy of prematurity do not show an increase in cerebral palsy or cognitive dysfunction. Therefore, although oxygenation state and hypocapnia are factors that could conceivably contribute to LD after anesthesia, experimental support for this possibility is not robust, although future animal studies could evaluate this possibility.

We appreciate the opportunity to respond to the thoughtful concerns and criticism contained in the accompanying letters. Each of the authors has provided additional food for thought as this issue moves forward. What unifies all is the clear need for larger, more extensive prospective and retrospective studies that would allow for the control of comorbidity and variations in anesthetic management, the examination of effects according to surgical procedure, the determination of effect by LD type, and more comprehensive measures of academic achievement, cognitive/memory functions, and quality of life. This study represents an initial attempt at unraveling this complex and difficult issue. Other studies planned and currently under way will, no doubt, add to the slowly accumulating body of clinical data that we hope will help to resolve this important and difficult issue.

*Mayo Clinic, Rochester, Minnesota. sprung.juraj@mayo.edu

1.
Wilder RT, Flick RP, Sprung J, Katusic SK, Barbaresi WJ, Mickelson C, Gleich SJ, Schroeder DR, Weaver AL, Warner DO: Early exposure to anesthesia and learning disabilities in a population-based birth cohort. Anesthesiology 2009; 110:796–804
2.
Loepke AW, McGowan FX Jr, Soriano SG: Con: The toxic effects of anesthetics in the developing brain—The clinical perspective. Anesth Analg 2008; 106:1664–9
3.
Jevtovic-Todorovic V, Olney JW: Pro: Anesthesia-induced developmental neuroapoptosis—Status of the evidence. Anesth Analg 2008; 106:1659–63
4.
Sun L: Anesthesia and neurodevelopment in children: Time for an answer. Anesthesiology 2008; 109:5
5.
Soriano SG, Anand KJ, Rovnaghi CR, Hickey PR: Of mice and men: Should we extrapolate rodent experimental data to the care of human neonates? (letter). Anesthesiology 2005; 102:866–8, reply 868–9
6.
Olney JW, Young C, Wozniak DF, Ikonomidou C, Jevtovic-Todorovic V: Anesthesia-induced developmental neuroapoptosis. Does it happen in humans? Anesthesiology 2004; 101:273–5
7.
Hansen TG, Danish Registry Study Group, Flick R, Mayo Clinic Pediatric Anesthesia and Learning Disabilities Study Group: Anesthetic effects on the developing brain: Insights from epidemiology. Anesthesiology 2009; 110:1–3
Danish Registry Study Group
Mayo Clinic Pediatric Anesthesia and Learning Disabilities Study Group
8.
Mitchell RB, Kelly J: Behavior, neurocognition and quality-of-life in children with sleep-disordered breathing. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 2006; 70:395–406
9.
Mitchell RB: Adenotonsillectomy for obstructive sleep apnea in children: Outcome evaluated by pre- and postoperative polysomnography. Laryngoscope 2007; 117:1844–54
10.
Hutton JB: Incidence of learning problems among children with middle ear pathology. J Learn Disabil 1984; 17:41–2
11.
Campbell TF, Dollaghan CA, Rockette HE, Paradise JL, Feldman HM, Shriberg LD, Sabo DLL, Kurs-Lasky M: Risk factors for speech delay of unknown origin in 3-year-old children. Child Dev 2003; 74:346–57
12.
Cote CJ, Rolf N, Liu LM, Goudsouzian NG, Ryan JF, Zaslavsky A, Gore R, Todres TD, Vassallo S, Polaner D, Alifimoff JK: A single-blind study of combined pulse oximetry and capnography in children. Anesthesiology 1991; 74:980–7
13.
Deulofeut R, Critz A, Adams-Chapman I, Sola A: Avoiding hyperoxia in infants < or = 1250 g is associated with improved short- and long-term outcomes. J Perinatol 2006; 26:700–5
14.
Tin W, Milligan DW, Pennefather P, Hey E: Pulse oximetry, severe retinopathy, and outcome at one year in babies of less than 28 weeks gestation. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 2001; 84:F106–10