Background

Dream recall is reportedly more common after propofol than after volatile anesthesia, but this may be due to delayed emergence or more amnesia after longer-acting volatiles. The electroencephalographic signs of dreaming during anesthesia and the differences between propofol and desflurane also are unknown. The authors therefore compared dream recall after propofol- or desflurane-maintained anesthesia and analyzed electroencephalographic patterns in dreamers and nondreamers and in propofol and desflurane patients for similarities to rapid eye movement and non-rapid eye movement sleep.

Methods

Three hundred patients presenting for noncardiac surgery were randomized to receive propofol- or desflurane-maintained anesthesia. The raw electroencephalogram was recorded from induction until patients were interviewed about dreaming when they became first oriented postoperatively. Using spectral and ordinal methods, the authors quantified the amount of sleep spindle-like activity and high-frequency power in the electroencephalogram.

Results

The incidence of dream recall was similar for propofol (27%) and desflurane (28%) patients. Times to interview were similar (median 20 [range 4-114] vs. 17 [7-86] min; P = 0.1029), but bispectral index values at interview were lower (85 [69-98] vs. 92 [40-98]; P < 0.0001) in propofol than in desflurane patients. During surgery, the raw electroencephalogram of propofol patients showed more and faster spindle activity than in desflurane patients (P < 0.001). The raw electroencephalogram of dreamers showed fewer spindles and more high-frequency power than in nondreamers in the 5 min before interview (P < 0.05).

Conclusions

Anesthetic-related dreaming seems to occur just before awakening and is associated with a rapid eye movement-like electroencephalographic pattern.

PATIENTS frequently report that they have been dreaming during  anesthesia. However, dreaming may actually occur as patients recover from anesthesia, when the brain is still affected by sedative concentrations of anesthetic drugs that are sufficient to activate endogenous sleep mechanisms and the patients enter a sleep state.1–3If this hypothesis is correct, the raw electroencephalogram of patients who report dreaming may display characteristics of the raw electroencephalogram of natural sleep.

Dreaming occurs in both rapid eye movement (REM) and non-REM natural sleep. The electroencephalogram of non-REM sleep is characterized by loss of high-frequency electroencephalographic activity, the presence of sleep spindles (waxing and waning oscillations in the α[8–16 Hz] frequency range), K-complexes, and a varying amount of Δ (0.5–4 Hz) activity. In contrast, the electroencephalogram of REM sleep is very similar to the awake state (i.e. , broad-band high-frequency activity and lack of sleep spindles). REM sleep may be distinguished from the awake state by the presence of low muscle tone, rapid eye movements, and θ waves. The electroencephalographic signs of dreaming during natural sleep are not well described, but dream recall may be associated with more high-frequency activity4and suppression of α power.5–8However, the electroencephalographic patterns associated with anesthetic dreaming are unknown.

Patients receiving propofol for maintenance of general anesthesia often report higher incidences of dreaming than patients maintained with volatile anesthetics.9–12One explanation is that propofol and volatile anesthetics have different pharmacological effects in the central nervous system.11,13,14An alternative explanation is that propofol is associated with more rapid emergence from anesthesia than the older volatile anesthetics,15allowing patients to report their dreams before they are forgotten. Luginbühl et al.  16compared patients receiving propofol and desflurane (a volatile agent with a more rapid recovery profile) and reported no difference in dream recall between the groups. However, the incidence of dreaming was low (3% overall) because patients were not interviewed until the first postoperative day.

To investigate these hypotheses further, we randomized patients to propofol or desflurane-maintained anesthesia and collected raw electroencephalogram from induction of anesthesia until completion of an early postoperative interview. Specifically, we tested the hypotheses that in patients presenting for noncardiac surgery under relaxant general anesthesia (1) propofol maintenance is associated with a higher incidence of dream recall than desflurane maintenance, (2) the raw electroencephalogram distinguishes patients who report dreaming from those who do not, and (3) the raw electroencephalogram distinguishes patients receiving propofol and desflurane.

This randomized, double-blind controlled trial received prospective ethics committee approval at the Royal Melbourne Hospital, Melbourne, Australia; Royal Perth Hospital, Perth, Australia; King Edward Hospital for Women, Perth, Australia; and Waikato Hospital, Hamilton, New Zealand.

Eligible patients were aged 18–50 yr, were American Society of Anesthesiologists’ physical status I–III, and were scheduled for elective noncardiac surgery under relaxant general anesthesia. Patients with inadequate English comprehension due to a language barrier, cognitive deficit, or intellectual disability, psychotic disorders, major affective disorders, or major drug abuse, or taking a benzodiazepine or more than two standard alcoholic drinks on the evening before surgery were excluded. Written informed consent was obtained from all recruited patients.

The primary endpoint was the incidence of dreaming reported on emergence from general anesthesia. Dreaming during anesthesia was defined as any experience that was described by the patient as dreaming and was thought by the patient to have occurred between induction of anesthesia and emergence after anesthesia. In our previous study, dreaming was reported on emergence in 36% of propofol patients and 20% of desflurane patients.1A sample size of 270 patients (135 patients per group) provides 80% power to detect this difference (36% vs.  20%; α= 0.05). We therefore planned to recruit 300 patients in total. With 300 patients, the power to detect a 2-min difference between the two groups in the secondary endpoint of time to eye opening was 80% (14 min vs.  12 min; SD = 6 min; n1 = n2 = 150).

Patients were randomized from a computer-generated list**(block randomized by site), and randomization results were concealed until after consent was obtained. Patients were blind to group allocation. Intravenous access and routine monitoring were established. After skin preparation, a bispectral index (BIS) sensor (BIS-XP; Aspect Medical Systems Inc, Norwood, MA) was applied to the forehead of all patients, and electroencephalographic recording commenced. Anesthesia was induced with 1–2 μg/kg fentanyl, propofol, and a muscle relaxant and was maintained with the randomized maintenance agent. In the propofol group, a target-controlled infusion device was used to target desired plasma propofol concentrations. In the desflurane group, desflurane administration commenced after induction with propofol. Anesthesia was titrated to BIS 40–55 during maintenance. Morphine, paracetamol, nonsteroidal antiinflammatory drugs, local anesthetic infiltration, peripheral nerve blocks, dexamethasone, and/or a 5-hydroxytryptamine3receptor blocker were allowed, but other opioids, nitrous oxide, midazolam, tramadol, ketamine, and major plexus and neuraxial blockade were prohibited. At the conclusion of surgery, after reversal of neuromuscular blockade and tracheal extubation, patients were taken to the postanesthesia care unit (PACU).

Patients were interviewed as soon as they became oriented to time, place, and person. We used the following standard questionnaire.17What was the last thing you remember before going to sleep? What was the first thing you remember when you woke up? Can you recall anything between? Did you have any dreams during your anesthetic? Electroencephalographic recording continued until after the interview was complete. Interviewers were blind to group allocation and electroencephalographic data. If dreaming was reported, a narrative report was collected. All patients who reported dreaming were considered to be dreamers for the purpose of the analyses, whether or not they could remember the narrative of the dream.

Data Collection

Baseline data included demographic and surgical details, home dreaming recall frequency (0 = never; 1 = less than once a week; 2 = several times a week; 3 = almost every morning) and risk factors for awareness, including a past history of awareness, heavy alcohol or sedative drug use and anticipated difficult intubation. Clinical signs of inadequate anesthesia, including movement and autonomic signs (tachycardia, hypertension, sweating, and lacrimation) were recorded. Anesthesia duration was defined as the time from induction of anesthesia to the completion of wound closure. Times to eye opening, to orientation to time, place, and person, and to eligibility for PACU discharge (Aldrete score ≥ 918) commenced at time of completion of wound closure.

Raw electroencephalographic data were collected in real time, and BIS data were downloaded from the monitor at the end of each case, both with specific patient consent and using research software provided by Aspect Medical Systems (once-per-minute recordings; smoothing = 15 s for BIS data; recordings with signal quality below 50 were removed from the analysis).

Electroencephalographic Analysis

The raw electroencephalographic signal was digitized at 128/s and 14-bit resolution. The signal was then bandpass filtered between 1 and 41 Hz by using a ninth order Butterworth filter. Segments with a maximum amplitude greater than 200 μV were rejected as artifacts. We hypothesized that recalled dreams would occur close to the time of awakening; therefore, we concentrated the analysis on this period. Specifically, we analyzed 30-s segments of the electroencephalogram at the following time points: (1) the middle of the operation, (2) completion of wound closure, (3) eye opening, and (4) a sequence of times (−1 min, −2 min, −3 min … up to −20 min) before the dream interview.

The electroencephalographic waveforms were characterized in two different ways: spectral and ordinal. The power spectral density of the electroencephalographic segment was estimated by using the absolute value of the complex number that is output from the ‘psd.m’ Matlab function (Matlab 7.7.0; The Mathworks Inc, Natick, MA). We used this function because it is the standard nonparametric method, and it makes no assumptions about system linearity or noise inputs. It applies the Welch method to obtain averaged periodograms (by using a tapered Hamming window of length 64, 50% overlap, and 1-Hz frequency resolution). We attempted to develop a succinct description of the electroencephalographic power spectrum by using only a few parameters. To do this, it is necessary to separate the narrow-band oscillations in the electroencephalographic signal from the underlying broadband irregular activity. After taking the natural logarithm of the spectral density, this background activity was quantified by fitting a linear regression to the subset of frequencies in which there were no prominent oscillatory peaks. In our case, we used the frequencies that avoided the Δ (1–4 Hz) and α (8–16 Hz) oscillations – namely the 5–7 Hz and 17–35 Hz ranges. The strength of narrow-band oscillations was quantified by finding the peaks in the α and Δ wavebands (fig. 1). θ oscillations did not interfere with this process. Thus, the electroencephalographic spectrum could be described by seven parameters: the broadband activity by the regression line slope and intercept, and the narrow-band oscillations by the height and frequency of the α and Δ peaks, and the spindle amplitude (the distance between the α peak and regression line underneath [the length of the vertical dotted line in fig. 1]). The power spectrum of natural non-REM sleep would be expected to show strong narrow-band oscillations in the α or Δ bands and a steep slope for the regression fit to the background activity. Conversely, the power spectrum of REM sleep would be expected to show no α or Δ oscillations and a flat gradient for the background activity.

Fig. 1. Illustration of the method of obtaining parameters from the electrocephalographic power spectrum. The power spectrum is calculated as described in the text. The regression line describes the underlying broad band activity, and it is fitted to the power at frequencies (5–7 and 17–35 Hz). 

Fig. 1. Illustration of the method of obtaining parameters from the electrocephalographic power spectrum. The power spectrum is calculated as described in the text. The regression line describes the underlying broad band activity, and it is fitted to the power at frequencies (5–7 and 17–35 Hz). 

Close modal

A major problem with the use of Fourier methods of analysis for nonsinusoidal signals like the electroencephalogram is the presence of harmonics. For example, the spectral power at 16 Hz could reflect a pure 16 Hz sine-wave oscillation; alternatively, it could be generated by the second harmonic of a more angularly shaped 8-Hz oscillation. Bispectral analysis is suitable for detection and quantification of higher harmonics and is used in the BIS algorithm for this purpose. Another solution is to use an ordinal analysis that simply detects a sequence of peaks and troughs at the prescribed wavelength/“frequency” in the electroencephalographic signal. This method makes no assumptions about the shape or size of the peaks and troughs (as long as they are greater than a preset minimum threshold amplitude). This method is useful in the detection of oscillatory spindle-like activity in the electroencephalogram. The output from this method is the percentage of sampled data points that could be associated with a short (/\/\/, 2peak-2trough) electroencephalographic pattern at the specific frequencies (10.68 Hz, 12.82 Hz, 16.02 Hz, and 21.36 Hz). We therefore used the ordinal method of analysis to complement (and check) the power spectral methods. During the course of the analysis, we tried various combinations of different settings for the spindle method (longer spindle sequences [up to 11 peaks and troughs] and different noise thresholds), but we found no improvement in discriminatory power.

Statistical Analyses

Continuous variables were graphed to assess their distribution. Normally distributed variables were described by using mean and SD and compared using two-tailed Student t  tests. Skewed variables were described by using median and range and compared by using Wilcoxon rank sum test. Categorical variables were described by using number (%) and compared by using chi-square or Fisher exact test. Survival data (time to an event) were assessed by using log-rank tests. To minimize the biasing effect of a few outliers, the electroencephalographic parameters are presented as median (interquartile range). Predictors of dreaming from univariate analyses with P  values less than 0.2 were all included in multivariate logistic regression models. Backwards elimination was used to eliminate nonsignificant predictors and to create parsimonious models. Interactions were tested (none significant – not shown). Results of these analyses are presented as adjusted odds ratios (OR) and 95% confidence intervals (CI). Statistical analyses were performed using Stata 8.2 (Stata Corporation, College Station, TX) and Matlab 7.7.0. P < 0.05 was accepted as statistically significant.

A total of 330 patients consented to participation, and 300 were randomized. Thirty consenting patients were not randomized because their surgery was cancelled or rescheduled or because the anesthetic plan changed. Patients were similar at baseline (table 1). There was unequal gender distribution between the sites (100% female at King Edward Memorial Hospital for Women, 95% female at Waikato, 58% female at Royal Melbourne Hospital, and 58% female at Royal Perth Hospital), but there were no statistically significant differences in the incidence of dreaming (38%, 24%, 23%, and 38%, respectively; P = 0.07). Patients randomized to propofol maintenance received higher total fentanyl doses and were more likely to move during anesthesia than patients randomized to desflurane (table 2). The range of BIS values during anesthesia was greater in propofol patients, and propofol patients had lower BIS values at eye opening and at the postoperative interview (table 3). Loss of BIS data because of signal quality less than 50 was not different between the propofol and desflurane groups during maintenance (1% [interquartile range 0–3%]vs.  0% [0–4%]; P = 0.47) or recovery (0% [0–13%]vs.  0% [0–7%]; P = 0.20).

Table 1. Baseline Characteristics of Patients Randomized to Propofol or Desflurane 

Table 1. Baseline Characteristics of Patients Randomized to Propofol or Desflurane 
Table 1. Baseline Characteristics of Patients Randomized to Propofol or Desflurane 

Table 2. Intraoperative and Postoperative Characteristics of Patients Randomized to Propofol or Desflurane 

Table 2. Intraoperative and Postoperative Characteristics of Patients Randomized to Propofol or Desflurane 
Table 2. Intraoperative and Postoperative Characteristics of Patients Randomized to Propofol or Desflurane 

Table 3. Bispectral Index (BIS) Values in Patients Randomized to Propofol and Desflurane 

Table 3. Bispectral Index (BIS) Values in Patients Randomized to Propofol and Desflurane 
Table 3. Bispectral Index (BIS) Values in Patients Randomized to Propofol and Desflurane 

Dreaming Results: Propofol versus  Desflurane

Dreaming was reported on emergence by 27% of propofol patients and 28% of desflurane patients. Patients reported simple dreams about family, friends, work, and recreation. No patients reported awareness during anesthesia, and there were no dreams that were suggestive of intraoperative memory formation. Dreaming patients had higher home dream recall (table 4) and higher BIS values at interview (table 5). Thirty-five percent of gynecological surgery patients compared with 20% of other surgery patients recalled dreaming (P = 0.08). The only significant multivariate predictors of dreaming were dream recall greater than 1 per week (OR 3.01; 95% CI 1.53–5.93; P = 0.001), anesthesia duration of no more than 100 min (OR 1.96; 95% CI 1.07–3.60; P = 0.03), and BIS greater than 90 at interview (OR 1.89; 95% CI 1.05–3.45; P = 0.035).

Table 4. Characteristics of Nondreamers and Dreamers 

Table 4. Characteristics of Nondreamers and Dreamers 
Table 4. Characteristics of Nondreamers and Dreamers 

Table 5. Bispectral Index (BIS) Values in Nondreamers and Dreamers 

Table 5. Bispectral Index (BIS) Values in Nondreamers and Dreamers 
Table 5. Bispectral Index (BIS) Values in Nondreamers and Dreamers 

Electroencephalographic Results: Dreamers versus  Nondreamers

Suitable raw electroencephalographic data were obtained from only 150 patients due to difficulty in downloading raw electroencephalographic data and in achieving adequate signal quality in the other patients. Patients without raw electroencephalographic data were similar to patients with raw electroencephalographic data: mean age was 35 ± 9 versus  37 ± 9 yr (P = 0.047), 68% of patients were female in both groups (P = 0.937), American Society of Anesthesiologists status was 2–3 in 46% and 68% of patients (P = 0.037), and dream recall was 32% versus  23% (P = 0.062).

We undertook extensive analysis to characterize the raw electroencephalographic waveforms so that any putative parameter found to discriminate between the groups (dreamers vs.  nondreamers and propofol vs.  desflurane) could be linked to the underlying neurobiology of anesthesia, sleep, and dreaming. We have not reported most of this work because the best discriminatory parameters were simple: the spectral power in the high frequencies (greater than 20 Hz) and the relative amount of spindle activity. These two parameters are easily linked to REM and non-REM electroencephalographic patterns.

The differences in electroencephalographic parameters between the patients who reported dreams (n = 34) and those who did not (n = 116) were small. During surgery, there were no significant differences between dreamers and nondreamers, except that at wound closure the electroencephalographic slope parameter was less for dreamers than for nondreamers; and the log spectral power at 30 Hz was 4.8 (4.2 to 5.1) μV for dreamers versus  4.4 (3.8 to 4.9) μV for nondreamers (P = 0.09) (table 6).

Table 6. Raw Electroencephalographic Parameters in Dreamers and Nondreamers 

Table 6. Raw Electroencephalographic Parameters in Dreamers and Nondreamers 
Table 6. Raw Electroencephalographic Parameters in Dreamers and Nondreamers 

A graphical demonstration of the mean changes in spectral power during the 20 min preceding the interview is shown in figure 2. Dreamers and nondreamers are represented by the colored and black mesh surfaces respectively. An 10-Hz spindle peak was lost as the patients approached the interview time, but the loss was more pronounced in the dreamers than in the nondreamers (i.e. , the black mesh surface lies on top of the colored surface in this part of the figure). Conversely, high-frequency power was greater in the dreamers than the nondreamers close to the interview time.

Fig. 2. Mean electrocephalographic power spectra of dreamers (  colored surfaces ) and nondreamers (  blue mesh surfaces ) in the 20 min before the interview. Both plots are of the same data, but they are rotated differently for ease of visualization. In the figures, the upper surface (which may be either  blue mesh or  rainbow color surface ) hides the lower surface at each point in time and frequency. 

Fig. 2. Mean electrocephalographic power spectra of dreamers (  colored surfaces ) and nondreamers (  blue mesh surfaces ) in the 20 min before the interview. Both plots are of the same data, but they are rotated differently for ease of visualization. In the figures, the upper surface (which may be either  blue mesh or  rainbow color surface ) hides the lower surface at each point in time and frequency. 

Close modal

More significant differences were observed just before the interview, when the electroencephalograms of dreamers showed more high-frequency (30 Hz) spectral power and fewer low-frequency (10.68 Hz) spindles (a REM-like pattern) than the nondreamers (fig. 3). There were no differences with respect to faster spindle oscillations.

Fig. 3. Changes in percentage spindles, high-frequency power, and eye opening in the 20 min before interview in dreamers and nondreamers. (  A ) Changes in 10.68-Hz spindles.  P values were as follows:  P = 0.01 (–1 min),  P = 0.0009 (–2 min),  P = 0.005 (–3 min),  P = 0.09 (–4 min) and  P = 0.04 (–5 min). (  B ) Log 30-Hz power.  P values were as follows:  P = 0.01 (–1 min),  P = 0.02 (–2 min),  P = 0.03 (–13 min),  P = 0.04 (–15 min) and  P = 0.03 (–17 min). (  C ) Percentage of patients who had opened their eyes at each time point.  P values were as follows:  P = 0.02 (–9 min),  P = 0.03 (–10 min). Results are presented as mean (SEM). * Significant difference (  P < 0.05) between the groups at that time point. 

Fig. 3. Changes in percentage spindles, high-frequency power, and eye opening in the 20 min before interview in dreamers and nondreamers. (  A ) Changes in 10.68-Hz spindles.  P values were as follows:  P = 0.01 (–1 min),  P = 0.0009 (–2 min),  P = 0.005 (–3 min),  P = 0.09 (–4 min) and  P = 0.04 (–5 min). (  B ) Log 30-Hz power.  P values were as follows:  P = 0.01 (–1 min),  P = 0.02 (–2 min),  P = 0.03 (–13 min),  P = 0.04 (–15 min) and  P = 0.03 (–17 min). (  C ) Percentage of patients who had opened their eyes at each time point.  P values were as follows:  P = 0.02 (–9 min),  P = 0.03 (–10 min). Results are presented as mean (SEM). * Significant difference (  P < 0.05) between the groups at that time point. 

Close modal

Electroencephalographic Results: Propofol versus  Desflurane

Propofol resulted in a more marked oscillatory peak in the frequency band 8–16 Hz (which corresponds to sleep spindle-like patterns) than desflurane (fig. 4), a more gentle underlying slope, a lower intercept, and larger and faster spindle frequency oscillations than the desflurane electroencephalogram (table 7). Desflurane patients had fewer spindle oscillations and more Δ power. There were no differences in the high-frequency components of the electroencephalogram. At eye opening, there were no significant differences between the groups, except that the peak frequency from the spectral analysis was 12 Hz for propofol patients and 9 Hz for desflurane patients (P < 0.0001) and the spindle amplitude for propofol was greater than for desflurane (1.49 [0.78 to 1.69]vs.  0.96 [0.69 to 1.66] log μV; P = 0.0002). The ordinal analysis of electroencephalographic segments from the middle of surgery provided similar results, showing that propofol was associated with increased spindle activity, particularly in the 10–12 Hz range (table 7).

Fig. 4. Mean power spectra of the propofol and desflurane groups at completion of surgical wound closure. 

Fig. 4. Mean power spectra of the propofol and desflurane groups at completion of surgical wound closure. 

Close modal

Table 7. Raw Electroencephalographic Parameters in Patients Randomized to Desflurane and Propofol 

Table 7. Raw Electroencephalographic Parameters in Patients Randomized to Desflurane and Propofol 
Table 7. Raw Electroencephalographic Parameters in Patients Randomized to Desflurane and Propofol 

This study found (1) no difference in dream recall between the propofol and desflurane groups, (2) more signs of cortical activation (more high-frequency power, fewer spindles, and higher BIS values) during recovery in patients reporting dreaming than not reporting dreaming, and (3) more spindle-like waves during maintenance of anesthesia with propofol than with desflurane.

Our finding of similar incidences of dream recall in patients randomized to propofol or desflurane is consistent with our hypothesis that the patients in the enflurane or isoflurane arm of previous randomized trials may have emerged more slowly from anesthesia and consequently had difficulty remembering any dreams.9–12Luginbühl et al.  16reported similar recovery times and similar incidences of dreaming in the propofol and desflurane arms of their study, which is consistent with our result. However, their overall incidence of dreaming was low because they did not interview patients immediately upon emergence from anesthesia.1Our finding contrasts with our previous cohort study in which the incidence of dreaming was significantly higher in propofol patients than desflurane patients (36% vs.  20%1). This difference may be attributable to selection bias in the previous study.

The apparent electroencephalographic activation in dreamers could be caused by awakening (responsiveness to external stimuli), REM-like dreaming (internal cognitive activity that is disconnected from external stimuli and associated with reduced responsiveness), or artifact from frontalis electromyographic activity. These three states are difficult to separate on the basis of electroencephalographic analysis because of the fluctuating levels of alertness and stimulation during emergence from anesthesia. However, sustained awakening or increased electromyographic activity are unlikely to be the cause of the activated electroencephalogram in our patients who reported dreaming; both dreamers and nondreamers were interviewed as soon as they were oriented, and there was no significant difference in the time from eye opening to interview between the two groups (table 4and fig. 3). We therefore conclude that the increased high-frequency power and decreased sleep spindle in patients reporting dreaming was due to a REM-like state.

This conclusion is further corroborated by the similarity between our electroencephalographic findings and those of researchers who wake up patients from natural sleep.5,6,8In these studies, suppression of electroencephalographic power in the α band was correlated with dream recall. During sleep states, α frequency oscillations consist mainly of sleep spindles, which are a definitive sign of stage 2 non-REM sleep. These spindles are generated in both natural sleep and general anesthesia, when neurons in thalamocortical networks become hyperpolarized and move to a burst-firing mode.19–21On the other hand, activation of endogenous cholinergic and aminergic neuromodulator systems results in cortical and thalamic neuronal depolarization, which reduces spindle activity in the electroencephalogram.21This might be why scopolamine prevents dream recall after anesthesia.22Our results suggest that patients who report dreaming lose their spindles in the recovery period to a greater degree than those who do not report dreaming. Whether the reduction in spindles is a direct electroencephalographic manifestation of the dreaming process itself or is an indication of likely improved dream recall cannot be determined from our data. The spindle activity found in stage 2 non-REM sleep is associated with impairment of memory consolidation; hence dreams are more difficult to recall after non-REM than REM sleep.4,23Our results support this idea.

We also found that the patients who reported dreaming had a consistent tendency to show more high-frequency power (20–40 Hz) in their electroencephalogram towards the end of their operation, even when they were still unresponsive. This is consistent with electroencephalographic findings in dreamers during natural sleep4and with the study by Aceto et al. ,24which found increased mid-latency cortical evoked potentials during anesthesia in patients who reported dreaming on emergence. Although the contribution of evoked potentials to the raw electroencephalographic signal is small, mid-latency evoked potentials contribute to the 20- to 40-Hz frequency band.

We also observed substantial differences in raw electroencephalographic patterns, but not in BIS values, between the propofol and desflurane groups. Our findings are of interest for two reasons. First, different electroencephalographic patterns for different drugs are a potential source of error in depth-of-anesthesia monitors. Of note, patients in the propofol group opened their eyes at lower BIS values than patients in the desflurane group, suggesting that the relationship between BIS and the clinical level of consciousness is different for the two drugs. Second, the differences in raw electroencephalographic pattern may be an indicator of differences in drug mechanisms of action. The propofol patients had larger and faster spindle-like patterns than the desflurane patients. Although the mechanisms of drug-induced spindles are only partially understood, the most parsimonious explanation is that propofol is a “cleaner” drug than desflurane. Presumably the sleep-spindle-like “oscillatory” electroencephalogram seen in our propofol group is largely the result of the γ-amino-butyric acid (GABA)-ergic actions of propofol, whereas the less well-defined, predominantly slower waves of the electroencephalogram in our desflurane group are caused by the additional (and unknown) receptor and ion-channel effects of volatile anesthetic agents (e.g. , potassium channel opening, N -methyl-d-aspartate receptor blockade).13,14 

Part of our study involved exploring which of the many possible electroencephalographic parameters would maximize the differences between the groups. For this purpose, we used several different analytic approaches (spectral and ordinal). The main problem with this approach is that any observed differences might have arisen by chance and may not be robustly reproducible. This is unlikely in the desflurane–propofol comparison, because the P  values were very low (≤ 0.001) and because we obtained similar results with different methods of analysis. In the dreamer–nondreamer comparison, the differences were much smaller. However, the changes were consistent over time and consistent with previously published work. We can be sure that our hypothesis that the dreamers might be in a slow-wave sleep state has been disproved.

Another potential limitation of this study is that patients in the propofol group exhibited more apparent variation in anesthetic depth than desflurane patients. This may be explained by the pharmacodynamic plateau effect that is seen in processed electroencephalographic variables with volatile anesthetics but not with propofol.25In addition, propofol patients received more opioids than patients in the desflurane group. This could have confounded the study because the electroencephalographic effects that we observed in the propofol group resulted from propofol combined with increased doses of opioids. Opioids are not characteristically associated with increased spindle activity in the electroencephalogram, but it is conceivable that their antinociceptive effects contributed to the increased spindle activity observed in the electroencephalograms of those in the propofol group. Finally, we could only obtain usable raw electroencephalographic data from half of the patients. These patients were not significantly different from the patients who were included; nevertheless, the power of our study to find differences between raw electroencephalographic parameters could have been limited. However, we can be confident that our study was adequately powered with respect to finding a difference between the incidence of dreaming in propofol and desflurane patients if one existed.

We conclude that reported dreaming during anesthesia is associated with more high-frequency power and fewer spindles in the electroencephalogram in the 5-min period just before recovery of full cognition. Whether these observations are the result of more actual dreaming mentation or less amnesia for the dreams is unknown.

The authors acknowledge the contributions of Jacklyn De Gabriele, R.N., Research Nurse, Department of Anaesthesia and Pain Management, Royal Melbourne Hospital, Melbourne, Australia; Susan March, R.N., and Shauna Fatovich, R.N., Research Nurses, Royal Perth Hospital; and Desiree Cavill, R.N., and Tracy Bingham, R.N., Research Nurses, King Edward Memorial Hospital for Women, Perth, Australia.

1.
Leslie K, Skrzypek H, Paech M, Kurowski I, Whybrow T: Dreaming during anesthesia and anesthetic depth in elective surgery patients: A prospective cohort study. Anesthesiology 2007; 106:33–42
2.
Lichtor J, Alessi R, Lane B: Sleep tendency as a measure of recovery after drugs used for ambulatory surgery. Anesthesiology 2002; 96:878–83
3.
Franks N: General anaesthesia: From molecular targets to neuronal pathways of sleep and arousal. Nat Rev Neurosci 2008; 9:370–86
4.
Williamson P, Csima A, Galin H, Mamelak M: Spectral EEG correlates of dream recall. Biol Psychiatry 1986; 21:717–23
5.
Bertolo H, Paiva T, Pessoa L, Mestre T, Marques R, Santos R: Visual dream content, graphical representation and EEG alpha activity in congenitally blind subjects. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 2003; 15:277–84
6.
Hong C, Jin Y, Potkin S, Buchsbaum M, Wu J, Callaghan G, Nudleman K, Gillin J: Language in dreaming and regional EEG alpha power. Sleep 1996; 19:232–5
7.
Morel C, Hoffmann R, Moffitt A: The electrophysiological correlates of dream recall and nonrecall from stage 2 sleep. Can J Psych 1991; 45:140–7
8.
Esposito M, Nielsen T, Paquette T: Reduced alpha power associated with the recall of mentation from stage 2 and stage REM sleep. Psychophysiology 2004; 41:288–97
9.
Marsch S, Schaefer H, Tschan C, Meier B: Dreaming and anaesthesia: Total Iv anaesthesia with propofol versus  balanced volatile anaesthesia with enflurane. Eur J Anaesthesiol 1992; 9:331–3
10.
Brandner B, Blagrove M, McCallum G, Bromley L: Dreams, images and emotions associated with propofol anaesthesia. Anaesthesia 1997; 52:750–5
11.
Kasmacher H, Petermeyer M, Decker C: Incidence and quality of dreaming during anaesthesia with propofol compared with enflurane. Anaesthetist 1996; 45:146–53
12.
Hellwagner K, Holzer A, Gustorff B, Schroegendorfer K, Greher M, Weindlmayr-Goettel M, Saletu B, Lackner F: Recollection of dreams after short general anaesthesia: Influence on patient anxiety and satisfaction. Eur J Anaesthesiol 2003; 20:282–8
13.
Grasshoff C, Drexler B, Rudolph U, Antkowiak B: Anaesthetic drugs: Linking molecular actions to clinical effects Curr Pharm Des 2006; 12:3665–79
14.
Rudolph U, Antkowiak B: Molecular and neuronal substrates for general anaesthetics. Nat Rev Neurosci 2004; 5:709–20
15.
Gupta A, Stierer T, Zuckerman R, Sakima N, Parker S, Fleisher L: Comparison of recovery profile after ambulatory anesthesia with propofol, isoflurane, sevoflurane and desflurane: A systematic review. Anesth Analg 2004; 98:632–41
16.
Luginbühl M, Wuthrich S, Petersen-Felix S, Zbinden A, Schnider T: Different benefit of bispectal index (BIS) in desflurane and propofol anesthesia. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand 2003; 47:165–73
17.
Brice D, Hetherington R, Utting J: A simple study of awareness and dreaming during anaesthesia. Br J Anaesth 1970; 42:535–42
18.
Aldrete J, Kroulike D: A postanesthetic recovery score. Anesth Analg 1970; 49:924–34
19.
Contreras D, Destexhe A, Sejnowski T, Steriade M: Spatiotemporal patterns of spindle oscillations in cortex and thalamus. J Neurosci 1997; 17:1179–96
20.
Fuentealba P, Timofeev I, Bazhenov M, Sejnowski T, Steriade M: Membrane bistability in thalamic reticular neurons during spindle oscillations J Neurophys 2005; 93:294–304
21.
Steriade M: Grouping of brain rhythms in corticothalamic systems. Neuroscience 2006; 137:1087–106
22.
Toscano A, Pancaro C, Peduto V: Scopolamine prevents dreams during general anesthesia. Anesthesiology 2007; 106:952–5
23.
Conduit R, Crewther S, Coleman G: Poor recall of eye-movement signals from Stage 2 compared to REM sleep: Implications for models of dreaming. Conscious Cogn 2004; 13:484–500
24.
Aceto P, Congedo E, Lai C, Valente A, Gualtieri E: Dreams recall and auditory evoked potentials during propofol anaesthesia. Neuroreport 2007; 18:823–6
25.
Kreuer S, Bruhn J, Larsen R, Grundmann U, Shafer S, Wilhelm W: Application of bispectral index and narcotrend index to the measurement of the electroencephalographic effects of isoflurane with and without burst suppression. Anesthesiology 2004; 101:847–54