Background

To assess whether patient sex contributes to the interindividual variability in alfentanil analgesic sensitivity, the authors compared male and female subjects for pain sensitivity after alfentanil using a pharmacokinetic-pharmacodynamic modeling approach.

Methods

Healthy volunteers received a 30-min alfentanil or placebo infusion on two occasions. Analgesia was measured during the subsequent 6 h by assaying tolerance to transcutaneous electrical stimulation (eight men and eight women) of increasing intensity or using visual analog scale scores during treatment with noxious thermal heat (five men and five women). Sedation was concomitantly measured. Population pharmacokinetic-pharmacodynamic models were applied to the analgesia and sedation data using NONMEM. For electrical pain, the placebo and alfentanil models were combined post hoc.

Results

Alfentanil and placebo analgesic responses did not differ between sexes. The placebo effect was successfully incorporated into the alfentanil pharmacokinetic-pharmacodynamic model and was responsible for 20% of the potency of alfentanil. However, the placebo effect did not contribute to the analgesic response variability. The pharmacokinetic-pharmacodynamic analysis of the electrical and heat pain data yielded similar values for the potency parameter, but the blood-effect site equilibration half-life was significantly longer for electrical pain (7-9 min) than for heat pain (0.2 min) or sedation (2 min).

Conclusions

In contrast to the ample literature demonstrating sex differences in morphine analgesia, neither sex nor subject expectation (i.e., placebo) contributes to the large between-subject response variability with alfentanil analgesia. The difference in alfentanil analgesia onset and offset between pain tests is discussed.

ALL anesthesiologists are aware of the large between-patient variability in the intended and adverse effects of most drugs they use in clinical practice. To achieve optimal anesthesia and (postoperative) analgesia, it is important to gain knowledge regarding those variables that contribute to this variability. An increasing number of experimental and clinical studies have demonstrated the importance of subject and patient sex in the variability of drug-induced anesthesia. For example, recent studies have shown that sex has a significant influence on propofol–alfentanil anesthesia sleep time (with faster emergence in women) and on morphine analgesia.1,2Morphine-linked sex differences are related to differences in morphine pharmacodynamics with greater morphine potency in women, whereas the sex differences in recovery from general anesthesia may have both pharmacokinetic and pharmacodynamic origins. Here, we expand our knowledge on the effect of sex on opioid analgesia. We studied the influence of sex on alfentanil analgesia in a group of healthy young volunteers using a pharmacokinetic–pharmacodynamic modeling approach; analgesia was measured using an experimental transcutaneous electrical pain model used previously.2Our main objective was whether the increased opioid potency in women previously observed for morphine and certain κ-opioid receptor agonists also applies to alfentanil, a widely used selective μ-opioid receptor agonist. A second objective was to examine whether placebo responses could explain some of the variability in alfentanil responses. The placebo effect is a complex phenomenon that consists of many subjective components (e.g. , expectation, memory and experience, attention, suggestion, and conditioning) and physiologic components (related to the activation of opioid and nonopioid analgesic pathways).3,4The influence of the placebo response on opioid analgesia is seldom examined. To do so, we incorporated the observed placebo responses in the pharmacokinetic–pharmacodynamic modeling of alfentanil. In addition to sex, opioid analgesic responses have also been shown to vary with pain type.5,6That is, a given opioid may be more effective against particular nociceptive modalities than others, an effect that may be genetically determined.6In addition, sex and gender pain differences vary with pain modality.7Therefore, as a final aim, we compared alfentanil analgesia against transcutaneous electrical pain, as characterized by our pharmacokinetic–pharmacodynamic model, with another pain modality, noxious heat, as well as on opioid-induced sedation.

Subjects and Apparatus

Thirty-six healthy volunteers (18 men and 18 women, aged 18–28 yr, body mass index < 28 kg/m2) were recruited after approval of the protocol by the local Human Ethics Committee (Commissie Medische Ethiek, Leiden University Medical Center, Leiden, The Netherlands) and provided oral and written consent. All female volunteers were taking oral contraceptives. Subjects were asked to have a normal night of sleep and not to eat or drink for at least 6 h before the study. They were comfortably seated in a hospital bed for the duration of the studies.

Design of the Studies

Pain (Studies 1 and 2).

We used two distinct pain models to test the pharmacodynamics of alfentanil.

Study 1: Transcutaneous Electrical Stimulation.

In 16 volunteers (8 men and 8 women) we determined the influence of a 30-min infusion of alfentanil and placebo on pain tolerance induced by transcutaneous electrical stimulation (TES) during a 5-h period, using a randomized, double-blind, crossover design. The sample size was determined on the basis of the results on our previous study on morphine with α= 0.01 and β= 0.8.2In an additional group of 10 subjects (5 men and 5 women) we tested the effect of no drug on the development of pain tolerance during a 5-h period.

Transcutaneous electrical stimulation was applied to the skin overlying the tibial bone (shin bone) of the left leg via  two surface electrodes (Red Dot; 3M, London, Ontario). The electrodes were attached to a computer interfaced constant current stimulator, which was locally designed and constructed. The intensity of the noxious stimulation was increased from 0 mA in steps of 0.5 mA per 1 s with a pulse duration of 0.2 ms at 10 Hz (cutoff = 128 mA). The subjects were instructed to press a button on a control box when no further increase in stimulus intensity was acceptable to them (pain tolerance). When the subject pressed the button, the stimulus train ended, and the current was collected and stored on the hard disc of a computer for further analysis. Before the studies, the subjects were trained for approximately 1 h, during which several stimulus trains were applied. These data were discarded. Next, baseline values were obtained in triplicate. The averaged baseline value was used in the data analysis.

Study 2: Thermal Pain.

The effect of a 30-min infusion of alfentanil and placebo on the visual analog scale (VAS) score in response to a painful heat stimulus was assessed in 10 subjects (5 men and 5 women). The VAS value was scored by the subjects on a 10-cm paper scale that ranged from 0 (no pain) to 100 (worst possible pain). Heat pain was induced using the TSA-II device (Medoc Ltd., Ramat Yishai, Israel) running WinTSA 5.32 (-10) software (Medoc Ltd.). Using a 3-cm2Peltier element or thermode, the skin of the volar side of the left forearm was stimulated with a gradually increasing stimulus (0.5°C/s). The baseline temperature was set at 32°C. After the subjects were familiarized with the device and the VAS scoring, the VAS score to three heat stimuli was assessed with the following peak temperatures: 46°, 48°, and 49°C. The stimulus causing a VAS score greater than 55 mm was used in the remainder of the study. The test data were discarded. Next, baseline values were obtained in triplicate. The averaged baseline value was used in the data analysis. The volar side of the arm was divided into six zones and marked as previously described.8The thermode was moved from zone to zone between stimuli.

Studies 1 and 2.

During and after drug infusion, pain assessments were made at regular intervals at times t = 5, 9, 15, 19, 25, 29, 33, 38, 43, 48, 53, 60, 70, 75, 80, 90, 100, 110, 120, 130, 140, 150, 160, 170, 180, 190, 200, 210, 240, 270, and 300 min after the start of the drug infusion. A similar time schedule was applied in the no-drug study.

Subjects participating in the drug studies were tested twice, with at least 3 weeks between the alfentanil and placebo arms of the study. Drugs were presented to subjects in a randomized and blinded order.

Sedation (Study 3).

In addition, we assessed the effect of alfentanil on sedation in all 16 subjects participating in the TES study. They were queried at regular intervals (t = 0, 5, 15, 25, 35, 45, 60, 75, 90, 120, 150, 180, 210, 240, and 300 min) regarding the magnitude of sedation by means of a visual rating scale from 0 to 9 (0 equals no sedation and 9 equals the feeling that occurs just before falling asleep).

Alfentanil Infusion and Blood Sampling

After arrival in the research unit, an arterial line for blood sampling was placed in the left or right radial artery during local anesthesia. In the contralateral arm, an intravenous line was inserted for drug infusion. Placebo (0.9% NaCl in water) or alfentanil (Janssen-Cilag BV, Tilburg, The Netherlands; alfentanil HCl, 0.9% NaCl in water) infusion commenced at 10:00 am using a target-controlled infusion. A Psion palm-top computer (London, England) programmed with the population pharmacokinetic data set reported by Maitre et al.  9was connected to a Becton Dickinson infusion pump (St. Etienne, France), which contained either alfentanil (0.25 mg/ml) or saline. The subjects were infused for 30 min. The infusion was such that the target plasma concentrations of alfentanil (Ctarget) and placebo were 50 ng/ml from t = 0 to t = 10 min, 100 ng/ml from t = 10 to t = 20 min, and to 150 ng/ml from t = 20 to t = 30 min. Time t = 0 was the start of the infusion.

In the drug studies, blood sampling took place at times t =−10, 3, 5, 9, 13, 15, 19, 23, 25, 29, 31, 33, 35, 38, 43, 53, 60, 75, 90, 120, 150, 180, 240, and 300 min. In instances where blood sampling coincided with pain assessment, the pain test preceded the sampling. Plasma was separated within 15 min of blood collection and centrifuged for 10 min at 3,500 min−1. Plasma samples were immediately stored at −25°C until analysis. Plasma alfentanil concentrations were determined by capillary gas chromatography, as previously described, with some minor modifications. A sample of 0.5 ml plasma was used and was extracted with 5 ml pentane Baker Ultra-resianalyzed Baker No. 9333. The operating temperature of the column oven was 215°C, and the flow rate of the carrier gas (helium) was 6 ml/min. The coefficient of variation of this method was lower than 3% in the concentration range of this study.

Pharmacokinetic–Pharmacodynamic Data Analysis

The population pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics of alfentanil were determined sequentially with NONMEM version V, level 1.1 (software for nonlinear mixed effects modeling; University of California, San Francisco, California; 1999).10 

Alfentanil Pharmacokinetic Model.

The alfentanil infusion rates were obtained from the log files of the target-controlled infusion device and used as input of a standard pharmacokinetic model, which consisted of either two or three compartments to assess how the alfentanil concentration data were best described. The pharmacokinetic parameters (volumes and clearances) were assumed to be lognormally distributed across the population.

Alfentanil Pharmacodynamic Data Analysis.

To eliminate a possible hysteresis between opioid plasma concentrations, as described by the pharmacokinetic model, and analgesic effect, an effect compartment was postulated. This effect compartment equilibrates with the plasma compartment with a half-life t½ke0(blood–effect site equilibration half-life).

The response to the electrical noxious stimulation (identifying pain tolerance) was modeled by assuming that alfentanil attenuates the response to the stimulus by inhibition of signal propagation or central processing or both. As a consequence, stronger stimuli are needed before the subject presses the pain tolerance button. The attenuation (A) was described by an inhibitory sigmoid Emax model2:

where Ce(t) is the alfentanil effect site concentration, AC50is the effect site concentration causing 50% attenuation, and γ is a dimensionless shape parameter. Because a response of the subject occurs when his or her pain sensation exceeds the response threshold for pain tolerance, we use equation 2for the current at time t, E(t):

where E0is the baseline or predrug current.

The VAS score in response to heat pain was modeled as follows:

where VAS(t) is the observed VAS score at time t and VAS at baseline is the predrug VAS score.

For sedation, the probability of observing a sedation score greater than k (k ranges from 0 to 8) is assumed to be11 

where Ckis the concentration where P(sedation score > k) = 0.5. The Ckvalues for k > 1 were parametrized by Ck= Ck − 1·[1 +θk· exp(ηk)], which allows incorporation of interindividual variability while guaranteeing that Ckis an increasing function of k. The probability of observing sedation score k is then given by P(sedation score > k) − P(sedation score > k − 1) and P(sedation score = 0) = 1 − P(sedation score > 0). The likelihood of observing the set of sedation scores was maximized (using NONMEM) to estimate the model parameters.

Placebo and Combined Alfentanil–Placebo Data Analysis.

For the electrical pain data, the following placebo model was designed, consisting of two parts, an increasing part and a decreasing part:

where EP(t) is the placebo response, E0is the baseline (= predrug) current, and P(t) is the placebo effect; its denominator and numerator allow for an increasing and/or decreasing effect, respectively. M and M′ are the magnitude of the increasing/decreasing placebo effect, and α and β are time constants of these effects, respectively. The magnitude M is equal to the magnitude M′, but in this model, both magnitudes can differ individually because the interindividual variability (η values) were allowed to be different for M and M′.

Alfentanil and placebo components were combined in an additive fashion:

where P(t) is given in equation 5. The placebo parameters determined from the placebo data analysis were fixed in the combined model.

Statistical Analysis.

The pharmacokinetic and pharmacodynamic models were fitted to the data with NONMEM. The interindividual variability of the parameters was described by using an exponential variance model:

where ηiis the interindividual variability term, θtypicalis the population parameter, and θindividualis the individual parameter value. The intraindividual variability was described by a proportional error model for the pharmacokinetic data and an additive error model for the pharmacodynamic data (except for sedation); the SD of the residual error (σ) was allowed to vary interindividually according to equation 7. To determine whether some biologic factors may explain the analgesic response variability, we tested the following factors (covariates): sex, age, weight, and lean body mass. They were included by allowing different parameter values for binary covariates and a dependence via  a power law for continuous variables, e.g. , for volume 1:

Model selection was done on the basis of the log likelihood criterion (−2LL; a decrease of more than 6.6 is significant at the P < 0.01 level for one additional parameter) and visual inspection of the fits. Besides visual inspection, the goodness of fit was also assessed by the coefficient of determination (R  2).

Alfentanil Pharmacokinetics

Figures 1A, D, and Gshow the average measured alfentanil concentrations. The pharmacokinetic model parameters are collected in table 1. A three- rather than a two-compartment model best described the pharmacokinetic data. For none of the model parameters did inclusion of the covariates (sex, age, weight, height, and lean body mass) improve the model fits (P > 0.01). Inspection of the individual data fits (see figs. 2 and 3for examples) showed that the pharmacokinetic model adequately described the alfentanil data as further quantified by the median of the weighted residuals and median of the absolute weighted residuals, which were –0.232% and 4.26%, respectively. On average, our target-controlled infusion caused 40–50% greater alfentanil concentrations than predicted by the Maitre kinetic set.

Fig. 1. Alfentanil and placebo analgesic responses assessed by using two distinct pain models (transcutaneous electrical pain:  A   C ; thermal pain:  D   F ) and alfentanil- and placebo-induced sedation (  G   I ). Top graphs (  A ,  D , and  G ) are the mean alfentanil plasma concentrations ± 95 confidence intervals (CIs); middle graphs are the mean alfentanil responses ± 95% CIs (  B : electrical pain;  E : thermal pain;  H : sedation); bottom graphs are the mean placebo responses ± 95% CIs (  C : electrical pain;  F : thermal pain;  I : sedation). Pain tolerance values (electrical pain model) are relative to baseline. CP= plasma concentration; Num. = numerical; VAS = visual analog scale score. 

Fig. 1. Alfentanil and placebo analgesic responses assessed by using two distinct pain models (transcutaneous electrical pain:  A   C ; thermal pain:  D   F ) and alfentanil- and placebo-induced sedation (  G   I ). Top graphs (  A ,  D , and  G ) are the mean alfentanil plasma concentrations ± 95 confidence intervals (CIs); middle graphs are the mean alfentanil responses ± 95% CIs (  B : electrical pain;  E : thermal pain;  H : sedation); bottom graphs are the mean placebo responses ± 95% CIs (  C : electrical pain;  F : thermal pain;  I : sedation). Pain tolerance values (electrical pain model) are relative to baseline. CP= plasma concentration; Num. = numerical; VAS = visual analog scale score. 

Close modal

Table 1. Population Pharmacokinetic Model Parameter Estimates 

Table 1. Population Pharmacokinetic Model Parameter Estimates 
Table 1. Population Pharmacokinetic Model Parameter Estimates 

Fig. 2. Influence of alfentanil and placebo on electrical pain tolerance in three subjects. Best, median, and worst data fits are given for the acute tolerance data fits (  B ,  E , and  H ). Corresponding measured alfentanil plasma concentrations (conc.) and pharmacokinetic data fits (  A ,  D , and  G ) and placebo responses and data fits (analyzed with the placebo model;  C ,  F , and  I ) are included. 

Fig. 2. Influence of alfentanil and placebo on electrical pain tolerance in three subjects. Best, median, and worst data fits are given for the acute tolerance data fits (  B ,  E , and  H ). Corresponding measured alfentanil plasma concentrations (conc.) and pharmacokinetic data fits (  A ,  D , and  G ) and placebo responses and data fits (analyzed with the placebo model;  C ,  F , and  I ) are included. 

Close modal

Fig. 3. Male (  A ) and female (  B ) alfentanil analgesic responses to transcutaneous electrical stimulation. Values are mean ± 95% confidence intervals (CIs). Note the very large response variability in both sexes. There were no differences in analgesic effect between the two sexes. 

Fig. 3. Male (  A ) and female (  B ) alfentanil analgesic responses to transcutaneous electrical stimulation. Values are mean ± 95% confidence intervals (CIs). Note the very large response variability in both sexes. There were no differences in analgesic effect between the two sexes. 

Close modal

Alfentanil and Placebo Pharmacodynamics

The alfentanil and placebo analgesic and sedation responses are given in figure 1. Alfentanil and placebo produced a significant increase in pain tolerance as derived from the electrical pain test. The maximum pain tolerance values ± SDs observed were 2.71 ± 1.38 and 1.29 ± 0.30 relative to baseline for alfentanil and placebo (panels B and C). The values were reached at times t = 48 and 60 min, respectively. In the heat pain test, alfentanil caused a significant reduction in VAS score from 80 ± 5 mm to 29 ± 10 mm at t = 29 min (panel E). In contrast, placebo VAS values did not change over time (panel F). Alfentanil but not placebo produced significant sedation (average maximum score = 6.8 ± 2.1 at t = 25 min; panels H and I).

Study 1: Transcutaneous Electrical Stimulation.

The results of the pharmacodynamic analysis are summarized in tables 2 and 3. In figure 2, best, median, and worst alfentanil pharmacodynamic data fits are plotted (panels B, E, and H) together with the corresponding placebo fits (panels C, F, and I). The alfentanil t½ke0and AC50were 9 min (95% approximate confidence interval [CI], 4–14 min) and 133 ng/ml (95% approximate CI, 73–193 ng/ml), respectively. Parameter γ could be fixed to a value of 1 without significant deterioration of the fit.

Table 2. Modeling Analgesia: Population Pharmacodynamic Model Parameter Estimates 

Table 2. Modeling Analgesia: Population Pharmacodynamic Model Parameter Estimates 
Table 2. Modeling Analgesia: Population Pharmacodynamic Model Parameter Estimates 

Table 3. Modeling the Placebo Effect: Population Parameter Estimates 

Table 3. Modeling the Placebo Effect: Population Parameter Estimates 
Table 3. Modeling the Placebo Effect: Population Parameter Estimates 

The placebo analgesic response varied in time course and magnitude among subjects; some subjects showed a predominant increasing placebo response, whereas others showed an increasing response followed by a decreasing component (fig. 2). The population analysis indicated that there was a significant analgesic placebo response (table 3), which was in magnitude approximately one fifth of the alfentanil analgesic response. The parameters obtained from the separate placebo analysis could be fixed in the combined model without a significant decrease of −2LL. Fixing the placebo parameters and subsequently restricting the analysis to the alfentanil set provided stability in the combined model. Combining responses yielded a reduced alfentanil potency and reduced t½ke0by approximately 20%: AC50(typical value ± SE [%CV], 95% approximate CI) = 164 ± 41 [72%], 84–244 ng/ml; t½ke0= 7.1 ± 2.0 [76%], 5.1–9.1 min. Fixing parameter γ to a value of 1 had no significant effect on the data fits.

For all three performed analyses, none of the tested covariates caused an improvement of the model fits at the P < 0.01 level. This indicates that alfentanil and placebo responses did not differ between men and women. See also the averaged male and female analgesic responses to TES ± 95% CIs in figure 3.

In 9 of the 10 subjects participating in the no-drug study, the current at which pain tolerance was reached did not differ over time (currents at t = 0, 60, 120, 180, 240, and 300 min: 19.1 ± 4.4, 20.4 ± 6.3, 21.1 ± 7.4, 18.6 ± 5.8, and 18.5 ± 8.3 mA, respectively [values are mean ± SD; analysis of variance: not significant]). The mean coefficient of variation was 8.3%. In one subject, a linear increase in current was observed from 14.5 mA (t = 0) to 28.5 mA (t = 300 min).

Study 2: Thermal Pain.

The results of the pharmacodynamic analysis are summarized in table 2. In figure 4, best, median, and worst alfentanil pharmacodynamic data fits are plotted (panels B, D, and F). The parameter estimates were as follows: t½ke0, 0.2 min (95% approximate CI, 0–0.4 min); AC50, 141 ng/ml (95% approximate CI, 81–201 ng/ml); and γ, 2.0 (95% approximate CI, 1.5–2.5). None of the tested covariates caused an improvement of the model fits. An interesting observation was that 8 of the 10 subjects showed some period of hyperalgesia after termination of the alfentanil infusion (VAS score > baseline; see fig. 4for examples).

Fig. 4. Influence of alfentanil on the visual analog scale score (VAS) using the thermal pain model in three subjects. Best, median, and worst fits are given for the VAS data fits (  B ,  D , and  F ). Corresponding measured alfentanil plasma concentrations (conc.) and pharmacokinetic data fits (  A ,  C , and  E ) are included. 

Fig. 4. Influence of alfentanil on the visual analog scale score (VAS) using the thermal pain model in three subjects. Best, median, and worst fits are given for the VAS data fits (  B ,  D , and  F ). Corresponding measured alfentanil plasma concentrations (conc.) and pharmacokinetic data fits (  A ,  C , and  E ) are included. 

Close modal

Study 3: Sedation.

The scoring system (ranging from 0 to 9) enabled the adequate description of the subjective feelings related to alfentanil-induced sedation (scores were made over the whole range of levels). None of the subjects fell asleep during the study, and all were able to complete the scoring. Randomly selected examples of data fits of five subjects are shown in figure 5. Visual inspection showed that the model adequately described the data. Model parameter estimates are given in table 4. The value of t½ke0was 2.2 min (95% approximate CI, 0–4 min). The alfentanil concentration causing 50% effect (or a sedation score between levels 4 and 5) was approximately 75 ng/ml. Ckvalues indicate the alfentanil concentration at which the probability of observing a sedation score greater than k  was 50%. For example, the probability of a sedation score greater than 8 (i.e. , 9) was 50% at an alfentanil plasma concentration of 296.4 ng/ml (Ck= 8). Figure 5shows the probability of the occurrence of sedation scores 0–9 as a function of alfentanil effect site concentration. The large overlap among sedation scores is apparent. Some scores never reach the highest probability (scores 1 and 2) and hence are not likely to occur at any alfentanil concentration.

Fig. 5. (  A ) Probability of sedation scores 0–9 (0 = fully awake; 9 = maximal sedation) as a function of the alfentanil plasma concentration. The sedation scores 0–9 are given in the figure and correspond to the lines with increasing peak concentrations. (  B ) Examples of the sedation data fits of five randomly selected subjects (subject numbers are shown). The sedation scale ranged from 0 (= fully awake) to 9 (= maximal sedation) as scored by the subjects themselves. 

Fig. 5. (  A ) Probability of sedation scores 0–9 (0 = fully awake; 9 = maximal sedation) as a function of the alfentanil plasma concentration. The sedation scores 0–9 are given in the figure and correspond to the lines with increasing peak concentrations. (  B ) Examples of the sedation data fits of five randomly selected subjects (subject numbers are shown). The sedation scale ranged from 0 (= fully awake) to 9 (= maximal sedation) as scored by the subjects themselves. 

Close modal

Table 4. Modeling Sedation: Population Pharmacodynamic Model Parameter Estimates 

Table 4. Modeling Sedation: Population Pharmacodynamic Model Parameter Estimates 
Table 4. Modeling Sedation: Population Pharmacodynamic Model Parameter Estimates 

Electrical Pain–Thermal Pain–Sedation.

In figure 6, we plotted the steady state data for the three endpoints of our studies. Parameter A in plots A and B denote the attenuation of the response sensitivity to the noxious stimulus by alfentanil (see equation 1). The shape of the response curves is for all three measures comparable with similar alfentanil potency observed for electrical and thermal pain. To visually compare the delays in effect and verify the results of the pharmacokinetic–pharmacodynamic analysis, we plotted the population averages of the measured plasma alfentanil concentration versus  the three measured effects (fig. 7). For electrical pain, a wide clockwise hysteresis loop with maximum effect occurred when the plasma concentration decreased to 100 ng/ml (fig. 7A).

Fig. 6. Steady state relations of the influence of alfentanil on electrical pain, heat pain, and sedation. On the y-axis, parameter A (see equation 2;  A and  B ) is given for both pain measures and expected sedation level is given for sedation (  C ). Ce= effect concentration. 

Fig. 6. Steady state relations of the influence of alfentanil on electrical pain, heat pain, and sedation. On the y-axis, parameter A (see equation 2;  A and  B ) is given for both pain measures and expected sedation level is given for sedation (  C ). Ce= effect concentration. 

Close modal

Fig. 7. Measured alfentanil plasma concentration  versus analgesia and sedation. Values reported are population averages. Error bars are omitted for clarity. (  A ) Analgesic effect in responses to pain from transcutaneous electrical stimulation shows a wide clockwise hysteresis loop. (  B ) Analgesic effect in response to thermal pain shows a small anticlockwise hysteresis loop. (  C ) Alfentanil-induced sedation causes a small clockwise hysteresis loop. CP= plasma concentration; VAS = visual analog scale score. 

Fig. 7. Measured alfentanil plasma concentration  versus analgesia and sedation. Values reported are population averages. Error bars are omitted for clarity. (  A ) Analgesic effect in responses to pain from transcutaneous electrical stimulation shows a wide clockwise hysteresis loop. (  B ) Analgesic effect in response to thermal pain shows a small anticlockwise hysteresis loop. (  C ) Alfentanil-induced sedation causes a small clockwise hysteresis loop. CP= plasma concentration; VAS = visual analog scale score. 

Close modal

Using a nonparametric loop collapsing approach on the data set of all individuals participating in study 1 yielded a t½ke0(mean [median]± SEM) of 11.3 [8.3]± 2.1 min.*For heat pain, little to no hysteresis was observed, and a small counterclockwise hysteresis was apparent. For sedation, a small clockwise loop indicating a small delay was visible.

We used a pharmacokinetic–pharmacodynamic modeling approach to compare the analgesic effect of the μ-opioid receptor agonist alfentanil in males and females. The main findings of our study are as follows: (1) Alfentanil is an equipotent analgesic in men and women; (2) placebo analgesic responses do not differ between men and women; (3) the placebo effect is successfully incorporated in the alfentanil pharmacokinetic–pharmacodynamic model and is responsible for 20% of the potency of alfentanil; however, the placebo effect does not critically contribute to the analgesic response variability; and (4) the pharmacokinetic–pharmacodynamic analysis of the transcutaneous electrical and heat pain data yields similar values for the potency parameter AC50, but the t½ke0is significantly greater when derived from electrical pain (7–9 min) than from heat pain (0.2 min). The smaller value of t½ke0corresponds with the small delay observed from sedation measurements (2 min).

Sex Differences

There is ample evidence from animal and human studies for the existence of sex differences in opioid-induced analgesia and adverse effects.2,7,12–18Approximately 70% of recent studies on opioid-induced analgesia in humans observed sex differences, with greater analgesia typically observed in women.19Discrepancies between studies may be related to the specific drug tested, dose, route of administration, pain modality, hormonal status of the subjects, and experimental design. Previously, we assessed the effect of sex on morphine analgesia in a study of similar size and in a population similar to the current one (young, healthy volunteers) using transcutaneous electrical stimulation.2We observed greater analgesic responses in women relative to men, which were related to sex differences in the pharmacodynamics of morphine and not to differences in pharmacokinetics. Our current data on alfentanil contrast with the observations made for morphine. Multiple factors may be involved in the difference between the current findings and our previous findings. Previously, we used transcutaneous electrical stimulation at a stimulus frequency of 1 Hz versus  10 Hz in the current study. Different stimulus frequencies may activate different pain pathways, which then may vary in their sensitivity to sex differences. In contrast to our current design, subjects were previously asked to state at which point they experienced pain tolerance. Although we took great care to avoid any bias (e.g. , subjects were queried by same-sex researchers), we are unable to exclude this factor. However, our previous findings are in accord with other studies on sex-related differences in morphine analgesia (and side effects) and clinical findings for greater morphine analgesia in women.14,15,19,20Apart from methodologic issues, the difference in study outcomes may be related to specific pharmacologic differences between alfentanil and morphine (e.g. , recruitment of different G proteins at the inner layer of the cell membrane). Further studies are needed to explain the important and clinically relevant difference in behavior of various μ opioids in men and women.

The Placebo Effect

Potential causes (apart from sex) for the large analgesic response variability are psychological factors such as patient (or, in this study, subject) expectations, experiences, suggestion, and conditioning. These phenomena play an important role in the development of an analgesic (or hyperalgesic) response to placebo.3,4,21–23For example, Amanzio et al.  4showed that after pharmacologic blockade of the placebo response with naloxone or after hidden opioid injections, the variability and effectiveness of the analgesic response were decreased. We performed a simultaneous analysis of the alfentanil and placebo responses to give us insight into the importance of placebo-related phenomena as a cause of the response variability as well as an indication of the “true” opioid potency. The placebo response has been incorporated before in pharmacokinetic–pharmacodynamic models of analgesia using both additive and multiplicative approaches (see Luginbühl et al.  5, Sheiner et al.  24, Mandema and Stanski,25and Romberg et al.  26for examples). Currently, we used an additive approach because placebo and alfentanil responses were not correlated (as determined from area under the effect response curves; data not given). The placebo response can be analgesic or hyperalgesic or may contain components of both, but usually, the hyperalgesic response is not acknowledged, and subjects who respond primarily hyperalgesically are considered nonresponders.22Our placebo model allows the placebo effect to decrease or increase with time, with different time constants for the increasing (α, equation 5) and decreasing components (β, equation 5) of the placebo response. Our placebo model is similar to one proposed by Mandema and Stanski.25The increasing and decreasing components of their model are additive with respect to baseline; in our model, they are multiplicative (and approximately additive on a log scale), which seems preferable because current cannot be negative. In contrast to what we expected, we did not observe a reduction in the variability of the estimated potency parameter obtained from the combined alfentanil–placebo model in comparison with the alfentanil model. This may be related to the very large analgesic responses after alfentanil relative to placebo (the AC50values indicate that placebo analgesia contributed only 20% to the total analgesic effect) and hence that the major part of the variability was in the opioid and not in placebo-related phenomena.

However, the following caveats should be noted. Although the alfentanil and placebo responses of single subjects were paired, the response of the subject (to alfentanil or placebo) may display large between-day variability. We did not take this into account. Further, the placebo analgesic response is not only related to μ-opioid receptor pathways but also to non–μ-opioid and nonopioid analgesic pathways.27These non–μ-opioid pathways may have interacted with the alfentanil response in a more complex fashion than described by our simple additive alfentanil–placebo model.

The absence of observing a significant difference between men and women in the placebo analgesic response is in accord with clinical studies using the post–third molar extraction pain model.28These data indicate absence of a sex dependency in the psychological and physiologic aspects of placebo responses, something that intuitively is not expected.28Because sex differences in analgesic responses are highly dependent on the specific pain model, significant sex differences in placebo responses may be visible in other pain models.

Alfentanil Onset and Offset Times

Alfentanil analgesic onset and offset (i.e. , t½ke0) are determined by multiple factors such as cardiac output, brain blood flow, transport across the blood–brain barrier, diffusion within the brain compartment to relevant μ-opioid receptor sites, receptor association and dissociation kinetics, activation of second messenger systems, and neuronal dynamics. Our estimate of the alfentanil t½ke0using TES (t½ke07–9 min) is large compared with values obtained from studies measuring the effect of alfentanil on the electroencephalogram, pressure pain, and pupil diameter (t½ke0approximately 1 min).29–32This prompted us to conduct additional experiments using a different pain model (heat pain) and to perform a post hoc  comparison of the pharmacodynamic parameters obtained from TES and heat pain models. We further performed an analysis on sedation data obtained in our initial study 1 (note that the sedation scoring scales were chosen to allow for the estimation of onset and offset times rather than to allow for an accurate description of the level of sedation). The estimated t½ke0values derived from TES differed significantly with the values obtained from heat pain (0.4 min) and sedation (2 min). In contrast, analysis of TES and heat pain data yielded a similar estimate of the analgesic potency of alfentanil (AC50values 130–140 ng/ml; table 2). This indicates that TES and heat pain models are comparable in steady state situations but differ under dynamic conditions. A similar “slow” alfentanil onset was observed previously using electrical stimulation of the finger and toe.33The slow TES response is not explained by a slow adaptation or habituation to the electrical stimulation, because no-drug studies showed constant TES response times during a 5-h measurement period in 9 of 10 subjects. Possible mechanisms for the relatively slow alfentanil response to TES include the following: (1) Different pain models may activate different pain pathways with differences in central processing (e.g. , due to differences in neuronal dynamics). It is possible that slower neuronal dynamics and activation of short-term potentiation at sites within the central nervous system involved in the processing of electrical pain responses may have caused the longer delay in alfentanil responses with TES, whereas no such slow dynamics were activated with heat pain. (2) A slow component in the TES response may be linked to placebo-related phenomena. Incorporating the placebo response in the alfentanil pharmacokinetic–pharmacodynamic model reduced the value of t½ke0by 2 min. Although 7 min is still a relatively large value, this observation suggests that at least part of the slow alfentanil response to TES is linked to placebo-related components. Our model may have been unable to extract the true magnitude of these components and hence may have overestimated the “true” alfentanil t½ke0. (3) In most of the heat pain (but not TES) responses, periods of hyperalgesia (i.e ., VAS scores > baseline) were observed during the slow decrease in alfentanil blood concentration (fig. 4). Under various circumstances, opioids may cause hyperalgesia due to activation of N -methyl-d-aspartate receptors.34A (slow) hyperalgesic component arising during heat pain testing (but not during TES) may have canceled out against a slow analgesic component, causing an overall short value of t½ke0in the heat pain experiments. In a preliminary analysis, we added a hyperalgesic component in the pharmacokinetic–pharmacodynamic model of VAS analgesia and observed a significant increase in t½ke0. (4) A combination of items 1, 2, and 3 may have caused the relatively slow alfentanil response to TES. Studies using the technique of functional magnetic resonance imaging may shed further light on the mechanisms of the observed slow onset and offset times of alfentanil observed with electrical pain but not with other surrogate measures of opioid effect (sedation/electroencephalogram, analgesia from heat and pressure pain).35 

Alfentanil Pharmacokinetics

A discrepancy was found between the target concentrations and the actually measured plasma concentrations related to a much smaller central volume (V1) in our population than provided by the pharmacokinetic set of Maitre et al.  9This discrepancy may be related to differences in the study population (in contrast to Maitre et al. , we studied a homogenous young population), infusion regimen, sample times, and duration of the studies. A three-compartment model described the pharmacokinetic data best, and the model output adequately characterized the observed plasma concentrations in this study (figs. 2 and 4). Neither sex nor age had a significant effect on the estimated pharmacokinetic parameters. This stands in contrast to a study from our institution showing a significant effect of age on alfentanil pharmacokinetic in surgical patients.36A possible explanation for the difference may be the small age range and the absence of underlying disease, surgery, and inflammation in the current study.

The goal of the current study was to increase our insight into the pharmacodynamics of potent opioids such as alfentanil, allowing us to optimize perioperative analgesic treatment. Although we believe that it is important to have knowledge on the potency and the onset and offset times of the opioids we administer, these population values will be of limited value when the determinants of variability are unavailable to predict the opioid requirements of individual patients. The current study indicates that sex and placebo-related phenomenon are unable to explain the large between-subject variability in alfentanil analgesic responses. The current absence of a cogent explanation for patient variability suggests that opioids are still best used by titration against analgesic effect.

1.
Gan TJ, Glass PS, Sigl J, Payne F, Rosow C, Embree P: Women emerge from general anesthesia with propofol/alfentanil/nitrous oxide faster thanmen. Anesthesiology 1999; 90:1283–7
2.
Sarton E, Olofsen E, Romberg R, den Hartigh, J, Kest B, Nieuwenhuijs D, Burm A, Teppema L, Dahan A: Sex differences in morphine analgesia: An experimental study in healthy volunteers. Anesthesiology 2000; 93:1245–54
3.
Amanzio M, Benedetti F: Neuropharmacological dissection of placebo analgesia: Expectation-activated opioid systems versus conditioning-activated specific subsystems. J Neurosci 1999; 19:484–94
4.
Amanzio M, Pollo A, Maggi G, Benedetti F: Response variability to analgesics: A role for non-specific activation of endogenous opioids. Pain 2001; 90:205–15
5.
Luginbühl M, Schnider W, Petersen-Felx S, Arendt-Nielsen L, Zbinden AM: Comparison of five experimental pain tests to measure analgesic effects of alfentanil. Anesthesiology 2001; 95:22–9
6.
Mogil JS, Kest B, Sadowski B, Belknap JK: Differential genetic mediation of sensitivity to morphine in genetic models of opiate antinociception: Influence of nociceptive assay. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1996; 276:532–44
7.
Craft RM, Mogil JS, Aloisi AM: Sex differences in pain and analgesia: The role of gonadal hormones (review). Eur J Pain 2004; 8: 397–411
8.
Cortinez LI, Hsu YW, Sum-Ping ST, Young C, Kiefer JC, MacLeod D, Robertson KM, Wright DR, Moretti EW, Somma J: Dexmedetomidine pharmacodynamics: II. Crossover comparison of the analgesic effects of dexmedetomidine and remifentanil in healthy volunteers. Anesthesiology 2004; 101:1077–83
9.
Maitre PO, Vozeh S, Heykants J, Thomson DA, Stanski DR: Population pharmacokinetics of alfentanil: The average dose–plasma concentration relationship and interindividual variability in patients. Anesthesiology 1987; 66:3–12
10.
Beal SL, Sheiner LB: NONMEM User’s Guide. San Francisco, University of California, 1999
San Francisco
,
University of California
11.
Somma J, Donner A, Zomorodi K, Sladen R, Ramsay J, Geller E, Shafer SL: Population pharmacodynamics of midazolam administrated by target controlled infusion in SICU patients after CABG surgery. Anesthesiology 1998; 89:1430–43
12.
Gear RW, Gordon NC, Heller PH, Paul S, Miaskowski C, Levine JD: Gender-difference in analgesic response to the kappa-opioid pentazocine. Neurosci Lett 1996; 205:207–9
13.
Gear RW, Miaskowski C, Gordon NC, Paul SM, Heller PH, Levine JD: Kappa-opioids produce significantly greater analgesia in women than in men. Nat Med 1996; 2:1248–50
14.
Dahan A, Sarton E, Teppema L, Olievier C: Sex-related differences in the influence of morphine on ventilatory control in humans. Anesthesiology 1998; 88:903–13
15.
Zacny JP: Gender differences in opioid analgesia in human volunteers: Cold pressor and mechanical pain. NIDA Res Monogr 2002; 182:22–3
16.
Mogil JS, Wilson SG, Chesler EJ, Rankin AL, Nemmani KV, Lariviere WR, Groce MK, Wallace MR, Kaplan L, Staud R, Ness TJ, Glover TL, Stankova M, Mayorov A, Hruby VJ, Grisel JE, Fillingim RB: The melanocortin-1 receptor gene mediates female-specific mechanisms of analgesia in mice and humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2003; 100:4867–72
17.
Kest B, Sarton E, Dahan A: Gender differences in opioid-mediated analgesia: Animal and human studies. Anesthesiology 2000; 93:539–47
18.
Craft RM: Sex differences in opioid analgesia: “From mouse to man.” Clin J Pain 2003; 19:175–86
19.
Miaskowski C, Levine JD: Does opioid analgesia show gender preference for females? Pain Forum 1999; 8:34–44
20.
Chia YY, Chow LH, Hung CC, Liu K, Ger LP, Wang PN: Gender and pain upon movement are associated with the requirements for postoperative patient-controlled iv analgesia: A prospective survey of 2,298 Chinese patients. Can J Anaesth 2002; 49:249–55
21.
Wager TD, Rilling JK, Smith EE, Sokolik A, Casey KL, Davidson RJ, Kosslyn SM, Rose RM, Cohen JD: Placebo-induced changes in fMRI in the anticipation and experience of pain. Science 2004; 303:1162–7
22.
Benedetti F, Amanzio M: The neurobiology of placebo analgesia: From endogenous opioids to cholecystokinin. Prog Neurobiol 1997; 51:109–25
23.
Benedetti F, Arduino C, Amanzio M: Somatotropic activation of opioid system by target-directed expectations of analgesia. J Neurosci 1999; 56:309–22
24.
Sheiner LB: A new approach to the analysis of analgesic drug trials, illustrated with bromfenac data. Clin Pharmacol Ther 1994; 56:309–22
25.
Mandema JW, Stanski DR: Population pharmacodynamics model for ketorolac analgesia. Clin Pharmacol Ther 1996; 60:619–35
26.
Romberg R, Olofsen E, Sarton E, den Hartigh J, Taschner PE, Dahan A: Pharmacokinetic-pharmacodynamic modeling of morphine-6-glucuronide-induced analgesia in healthy volunteers: Absence of sex differences. Anesthesiology 2004; 100:120–33
27.
Gracely RH, Dubner R, Wolskee PJ, Deeter WR: Placebo and naloxone can alter postsurgical pain by separate mechanisms. Nature 1983; 306:264–5
28.
Averbuch M, Katzper M: Gender and the placebo analgesic effect in acute pain. Clin Pharmacol Ther 2001; 70:287–91
29.
Scott JC, Ponganis KV, Stanski DR: EEG quantitation of narcotic effect: The comparative pharmacodynamics of fentanyl and alfentanil. Anesthesiology 1985; 62:234–41
30.
Glass PSA, Hardman D, Kamiyama Y, Quill TJ, Marton G, Donn KH, Grosse CM, Hermann D: Preliminary pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics of an ultra-short-acting opioid: Remifentanil (GI87084B). Anesth Analg 1993; 77:1031–40
31.
Egan T, Minto CF, Hermann DJ, Barr J, Muir KT, Shafer SL: Remifentanil versus alfentanil: Comparative pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics in healthy adult male volunteers. Anesthesiology 1996; 84:821–33
32.
Kharash ED, Walker A, Hoffer C, Sheffels P: Intravenous and oral alfentanil as in vivo probes for hepatic and first-pass cytochrome P450 3A activity: Noninvasive assessment by use of pupillary miosis. Clin Pharmacol Ther 2004; 76:452–66
33.
Coda BA, Cleveland Brown M, Risler L, Syrjala K, Shen DD: Equivalent analgesia and side effects during epidural and pharmacokinetically tailored intravenous infusion with matching plasma alfentanil concentration. Anesthesiology 1999; 90:98–108
34.
Eisenach JC: Preemptive hyperalgesia, not analgesia? (editorial). Anesthesiology 2000; 92:308–9
35.
Wise RG, Williams P, Tracey I: Using fMRI to quantify the time dependence of remifentanil analgesia in the human brain. Neuropsychopharmacology 2004; 29:625–35
36.
Lemmens HJ, Burm AG, Hennis PJ, Gladines MP, Bovill JG: Influence of age on the pharmacokinetics of alfentanil: Gender dependence. Clin Pharmacokinet 1990; 19:416–22