Background

It is not known whether epidural epinephrine has an analgesic effect per se. The segmental distribution of clonidine epidural analgesia and its effects on temporal summation and different types of noxious stimuli are unknown. The aim of this study was to clarify these issues.

Methods

Fifteen healthy volunteers received epidurally (L2-L3 or L3-L4) 20 ml of either epinephrine, 100 microg, in saline; clonidine, 8 microg/kg, in saline; or saline, 0.9%, alone, on three different days in a randomized, double-blind, cross-over fashion. Pain rating after electrical stimulation, pinprick, and cold perception were recorded on the dermatomes S1, L4, L1, T9, T6, T1, and forehead. Pressure pain tolerance threshold was recorded at S1, T6, and ear. Pain thresholds to single and repeated (temporal summation) electrical stimulation of the sural nerve were determined.

Results

Epinephrine significantly reduced sensitivity to pinprick at L1-L4-S1. Clonidine significantly decreased pain rating after electrical stimulation at L1-L4 and sensitivity to pinprick and cold at L1-L4-S1, increased pressure pain tolerance threshold at S1, and increased thresholds after single and repeated stimulation of the sural nerve.

Conclusions

Epidural epinephrine and clonidine produce segmental hypoalgesia. Clonidine bolus should be administered at a spinal level corresponding to the painful area. Clonidine inhibits temporal summation elicited by repeated electrical stimulation and may therefore attenuate spinal cord hyperexcitability.

Stimulation of alpha2-adrenoreceptors located in the spinal cord produces antinociception. [1] Therefore, epidurally administered adrenergic agonists have the potential to produce segmental analgesia.

The addition of epinephrine to local anesthetics or opioids may enhance epidural analgesia. [2–5] This effect may be the result of reduced vascular uptake of local anesthetics [6] and opioids, [4] which increases their concentration at the neural targets. Epinephrine is an alpha-adrenoreceptor agonist and produces antinociception when administered intrathecally in animals. [7,8] An early report described the efficacy of intrathecal epinephrine as a sole analgesic for labor in humans. [9] Analgesia after intrathecal administration does not necessarily imply analgesia after epidural administration. Systemic absorption, [10] dura penetration, and metabolism [11] may affect the spinal bioavailability of epidurally administered epinephrine. Segmental hyposensitivity to ice and pinprick has been observed after epidural epinephrine in one volunteer. [4] However, the effect of epinephrine has not been investigated in a randomized controlled fashion, so presently it is not known whether epidural epinephrine has per se an analgesic action.

Several investigations have shown the efficacy of the alpha sub 2 -adrenoreceptor agonist clonidine when administered epidurally for the management of postoperative [12–16] and chronic [17–20] pain. Epidural clonidine produces analgesia to cold pain test in the foot, but not in the hand, [21] which strongly suggests a spinal action. However, the segmental distribution of analgesia is not known, and the effects of epidural clonidine on different types of noxious stimuli and on temporal summation have not been investigated. Temporal summation occurs when the repetition of a peripheral stimulus causes increased pain perception. Increased and prolonged firing of dorsal horn neurons after repeated stimulation [22](central sensitization) is probably the mechanism underlying temporal summation. Central sensitization is likely to play an important role in the physiopathology of acute and chronic pain syndromes, such as postoperative and neuropathic pain. [23]

We have investigated the effects of epidurally administered epinephrine and clonidine on different types of experimentally induced painful and nonpainful stimuli applied on different dermatomes. The aims were to assess the segmental spread of analgesia, to identify the types of stimulation inhibited by epidural epinephrine and clonidine, and to investigate the effects of these drugs on temporal summation.

Anesthetic Procedure 

We studied 16 healthy volunteers. Exclusion criteria were a history of alcohol abuse or intake of psychotropic drugs, the intake of opioids or nonsteroidal antiinflammatory drugs in the past 2 weeks, the intake of other analgesics or sedatives in the past 24 h, coagulation abnormalities, pregnancy, or fever. The study was approved by the local ethics committee, and written informed consent was obtained from all volunteers. The investigation was conducted with a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, cross-over design. All randomizations were performed by drawing lots.

The volunteers fasted for at least 6 h and were not premedicated. Electrocardiograph, noninvasive blood pressure (one measurement every 5 min), and hemoglobin oxygen saturation using pulse oximetry (SpO2) were monitored with a Hellige Servomed monitor (Hellige AG, Freiburg, Germany). Expired carbon dioxide was monitored continuously via a nasal catheter with a Hewlett Packard M 1025B anesthetic gas analyzer (Hewlett Packard, Andover, USA). A peripheral intravenous cannula was inserted, and 4 ml/kg Ringer's lactate was rapidly infused, followed by 2 ml [center dot] kg sup -1 [center dot] h sup -1.

To minimize the risk of infection, bleeding, and perforation of the dura, no epidural catheter was inserted; only a single-shot injection was performed. For the same reasons, the three punctures were not performed at the same interspace, but one of two randomized schedules were followed: L2-L3, L3-L4, and L2-L3, or L3-L4, L2-L3, and L3-L4, with an interval of at least 1 week between sessions. All punctures were performed in the sitting position, with the median approach, using a 18-gauge Tuohy needle. They were done by the same anesthesiologist, who had previously performed more than 2,000 epidural blocks. The epidural space was identified by loss of resistance, injecting no more than 2 ml of 0.9% saline. Only a clear feeling of loss of resistance was considered as identification of the epidural space.

Each volunteer received, in a random order, an epidural injection of 20 ml of either epinephrine, 100 micro gram, clonidine, 8 micro gram/kg, (Catapresan[registered sign], Boehring, Basel, Switzerland) or saline, 0.9%, on three different days. Epinephrine and clonidine were diluted with 0.9% saline. The dose of 100 micro gram epinephrine was chosen because it is probably the dose most frequently used in association with local anesthetics for epidural anesthesia. The dose of 8 micro gram/kg clonidine was chosen because it roughly corresponds to the dose providing pain relief in postoperative patients. [12,15] A test dose of 5 ml of the epidural solution was first injected. After 4 min, the remaining dose was administered in increments of 5 ml every 60 s. Each 5-ml bolus was injected over 15–18 s. Before each injection, the syringe was detached from the needle to exclude intravascular or intrathecal placement. During this time, blood pressure was measured every 2 min. After injection of the last bolus, the volunteers were placed in the supine position.

Atropine, 0.5 mg, was injected intravenously if the heart rate was less than 45 beats/min. The volunteers scored sedation on a 10-cm visual analogue scale (VAS), where 0 = fully fit and 10 = hardy able to keep the eyes open. Blood pressure, heart rate, SpO2, end-tidal carbon dioxide, and sedation scores were recorded before and 30, 60, 90, and 120 min after the epidural injection and each time before the beginning of the test series.

Testing Procedure 

The three sessions were planned for each subject at the same time of the day. The volunteers first tried all tests for training. Then baseline recordings of all tests were performed. After the epidural injection, the test series were performed 30, 60, 90, and 120 min after the administration of the total volume of the solution investigated. In each series, the tests were performed in the following order: pain rating after electrical stimulation, pinprick, cold, pressure pain tolerance, and single and repeated electrical stimulation of the sural nerve. In all threshold determinations, the mean of two values was calculated. The test series lasted approximately 15 min.

Pain Rating (VAS) after Electrical Stimulation. Copper stainless steel dull pins (diameter, 1.2 mm; length, 2 mm) were applied on the left side to the skin of the following dermatomes: S1 (foot, just distal to the lateral malleolus); L4 (5 cm above a line drawn from the middle of the patella to the anterior superior iliac spine); L1 (anterior superior iliac spine); T9 (5 cm from the median line, on a horizontal line passing 3 cm above the umbilicus); T6 (5 cm from the median line, on a horizontal line passing 2 cm above the xiphoid process); T1 (elbow, on the medial epicondyle); forehead (1 cm above the middle of the eyebrow). A 25-ms, train-of-five, 1-ms, square-wave impulse (perceived as a single stimulus) was delivered from a computer-controlled constant current stimulator (University of Aalborg, Denmark). Subjective pain detection threshold, defined as the current intensity eliciting a distinct pricking pain, was determined at each dermatome tested in a randomized order before the epidural injection by increasing the current intensity from 1 mA in 1-mA intervals. For pain rating, which was the variable analyzed, a stimulus of an intensity of 1.8 times the baseline pain threshold (determined only before the epidural injection) was delivered at each dermatome tested in a randomized order. After each stimulation, the volunteers rated the perceived pain on an electronic 10-cm VAS. Before each rating series, two stimuli at two random dermatomes were delivered for training.

Pinprick and Cold. Sensitivity to pinprick and cold were tested at the aforementioned dermatomes, on the left side, 2 cm from the sites where the pin electrodes were applied. Pinprick was performed by pricking the skin twice with an interval of about 0.3 s, using a 21-gauge sharp-bevel needle. Cold sensitivity was tested with gel bags (Physiopack, Fisch Laboratories, Vibraye, France) kept in a freezer, and applied to 4 cm2skin surface for 2 s. Response was defined as hyposensitivity when subjects did not feel any sensation or when they felt only a sensation of touch or light pinprick (cold).

Mechanical Pressure. Pressure pain tolerance threshold was determined on the center of the pulp of the third and fourth left toes (S1), 3 or 4 cm above the xiphoid process (T6), and at the left and right ear lobes, in a randomized order. An electronic pressure algometer (Somedic AB, Stockholm, Sweden), [24] whose probe had a surface area of 64 mm2, was used. The pressure was increased from 0 at a rate of 30 kPa/s to a maximum pressure of 1000 kPa. Pain tolerance threshold was defined as the point when the volunteer did not want the pressure to be increased further. If the tolerance threshold was more than 1000 kPa, this value was considered as tolerance threshold. The mean of the two measurements at each dermatome was calculated.

Single and Repeated (Temporal Summation) Stimulation of the Sural Nerve. After the skin had been degreased, bipolar surface Ag/AgCl-electrodes filled with electrode gel (interelectrode distance, approximately 2 cm) were placed just distal to the right lateral malleolus. Electromyographic (EMG) reflex responses were recorded from the middle of the biceps femoris and the rectus femoris muscles (Ag/AgCl-electrodes). A leg rest was placed under the knee to obtain a 30 [degree sign] semiflexion. Electrophysiologic (flexion reflex) and psychophysical (perception of pain) thresholds were determined. The same type of stimulator used for pain rating was used. The EMG signal was amplified and filtered (1.5–150 Hz) by a Hellige (Hellige AG) single-channel EMG-electroencephalographic (EEG) amplifier. Stimulation and recording were controlled with the NFRsys software (Aalborg University) on a personal computer.

The impulse pattern described previously for pain rating was used. The current intensity was increased from 1 mA in 1-mA intervals until (1) a reflex with an amplitude exceeding 20 micro V for at least 10 ms in the 70- to 200-ms poststimulation interval was detected by the computer program (electrophysiologic single-stimulus threshold), and (2) a pain sensation was evoked (psychophysical single-stimulus threshold). To elicit temporal summation, the previously mentioned stimulus burst was repeated five times with a frequency of 2 Hz. [25] The current intensity was increased from 1 mA in 1-mA intervals until a summation threshold was observed. Summation threshold was defined as an increase in perception of current intensity during the five stimulations (psychophysical summation threshold) and an increase in the amplitude of the final one or two reflexes above a fixed limit of 20 micro V for at least 10 ms in the 70-to 200-ms poststimulation interval (electrophysiologic summation threshold;Figure 1). For single and repeated stimulation, if the threshold was above a maximal current of 80 mA, the threshold was defined as 80 mA.

Statistics 

Categorical and numerical data were analyzed by logistic regression [26] and two-factors multivariate repeated-measures analysis, [27] respectively. Both methods study the influence of various factors (independent or explanatory variables) on a dependent variable, which is dichotomous for logistic regression (e.g., hyposensitivity and normal sensation of pinprick) and numerical for two-factors multivariate repeated-measures analysis (e.g., pressure pain tolerance threshold). In the latter case, the experimental units were the 45 subject-drug combinations, and the other variables were treated as repeated factors. A P value < 0.05 was considered significant. The software used was SAS version 6.10 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC).

Eleven regression analyses were performed on the following dependent variables:(1) pain rating after electrical stimulation (VAS);(2) pinprick (hyposensitivity and normal sensation);(3) cold (hyposensitivity and normal sensation);(4) pressure pain tolerance threshold (kPa);(5) threshold after electrical stimulation of the sural nerve (mA);(6) pain during epidural injection (yes/no);(7) systolic blood pressure (mmHg);(8) heart rate (beats/min);(9) sedation score (VAS);(10) end-tidal carbon dioxide (vol %); and (11) arterial oxygen saturation (%). Numerical dependent variables were analyzed as the difference between values measured after administration of the epidural solution and basal values.

The independent (explanatory) variables included in the analyses of the aforementioned dependent variables were drug (placebo, epinephrine, or clonidine) for all analyses; dermatome where the stimulus was applied for the regressions on pain rating, pinprick, cold, and pressure pain tolerance; time (30, 60, 90, and 120 min after injection) for all analyses except for pain during injection (no time course), pinprick, and cold (risk of overfitting because of low frequency of positive outcomes); type of stimulus (single or repeated) and type of response (psychophysical or electrophysiologic) for the regression on threshold to electrical stimulation of the sural nerve. Potential interaction between drug, dermatome, and time have been investigated by including pairwise products of these variables (e.g., drug x dermatome) in the analyses (except for pinprick and cold, to avoid overfitting). For the analyses of pinprick and cold, data were stratified according to the sensitivity of subjects to the tests. All data collected from each subject in all three sessions, including all tests performed on all dermatomes at all times, were tabulated according to the response (i.e., hyposensitivity/normal sensation) in a 15 (subjects) x 2 (response) table. The subjects with similar frequencies of hyposensitivity to pinprick (cold) were grouped. Four groups for pinprick and two groups for cold were identified and included in the analyses as dummy variables. Because of the relatively low frequency of patients with hyposensitivity to pinprick and cold, in the regressions on these two variables, the dermatomes had to be grouped into two dummy variables: forehead-T1-T6-T9 and L1-L4-S1.

One volunteer was not enrolled because of abnormal prothrombin time. Of the 16 volunteers enrolled, one could not participate in two sessions because of fever and was excluded from the analysis. The remaining 15 (10 men) had a median age of 25 yr (range, 21–31 yr), a body weight of 71 kg (range, 52–86 kg), and a body height of 179 cm (range, 163–191 cm). Blinding frequently was impossible for clonidine because of the sedative effect caused by this drug.

Analgesia 

Clonidine, compared with placebo, significantly reduced pain rating after electrical stimulation, and its effect depended on the dermatome tested. When the dermatomes were separately analyzed, the effect of clonidine was significant at L1 and L4 compared with forehead (Figure 2, top). The effect did not change significantly during the testing procedure (variable time was not significant with any test used).

Epinephrine caused hyposensitivity to pinprick in 40% of subjects (P < 0.0001; placebo as reference) at the dermatomes L1-L4-S1 (P < 0.0001; group forehead-T1-T6-T9 as reference)(Figure 3, top). No significant effect of epinephrine was found with the other tests used.

Clonidine caused hyposensitivity to pinprick in 40% and cold in 30% of subjects (P < 0.0001 in both analyses; placebo as reference) at the dermatomes L1-L4-S1 (P < 0.0001 in both analyses; forehead-T1-T6-T9 as reference;Figure 3).

Clonidine, compared with placebo, significantly increased pressure pain tolerance threshold. The effect of clonidine was significant at S1 compared with ear, but not at T6 (Figure 4, top).

Clonidine, compared with placebo, significantly increased subjective and nociceptive reflex thresholds to single and repeated (temporal summation) electrical stimulation of the sural nerve (Figure 5). The threshold to nociceptive reflex was increased to a significantly larger extent than the subjective threshold.

Side Effects 

Clonidine, compared with placebo, significantly decreased blood pressure, heart rate, and SpO2, and increased end-tidal carbon dioxide and sedation score (Figure 6). Epinephrine significantly increased heart rate, compared with placebo (Figure 6). The maximum value of heart rate after epinephrine was 87; the third quartile was 69, and the median was 65 beats/min.

During the epidural injection, one subject reported nausea associated with bradycardia (lowest value, 48 beats/min) on two sessions (placebo and epinephrine). Another subject experienced the same symptoms (lowest value, 42 beats/min) during the injection of lidocaine in the interspinous space for local anesthesia. In all three cases, these symptoms were interpreted as vagal reaction and promptly reverted by the administration of intravenous atropine, 0.5 mg. In all other subjects, no change in heart rate or blood pressure more than 20% the baseline values were observed during the epidural injection, and no sign of intrathecal or intravascular placement of the epidural needle was observed. Pain during epidural injection was observed in 1 of 11 subjects with placebo, in 12 of 13 with epinephrine (P < 0.01; placebo as reference), and in 4 of 14 with clonidine (not significant). This usually was described as a feeling of pain pressure, which gradually decreased after injection and disappeared within 2–5 min. Data are not complete for all 45 sessions because this effect was not expected and was therefore not recorded on the first sessions.

Systolic blood pressure less than 90 mmHg (lowest value, 77 mmHg) was observed in five subjects, always after administration of clonidine. These values were slowly reached, i.e., no case of sudden hypotension was observed. Ephedrine was never administered. Atropine was administered intravenously in two subjects after clonidine administration because of bradycardia. The lowest value was 37 beats/min, observed after the end of the experiment. The intravenous injection of three doses of atropine, 0.5 mg, was necessary to increase the heart rate above 45 beats/min.

Fifteen minutes after injection of clonidine, one subject manifested an episode of apnea, as detected by the alarm system of the gas analyzer (which was activated after 30 s of apnea), associated with a decrease in SpO2to 80%. No sign of upper airway obstruction was observed. He was arousable by verbal command and could spontaneously breathe. After 2–3 s, however, he fell asleep and apnea manifested again. He had to be continuously aroused and incited to breathe. Because the subject was arousable and cooperative, the study was normally performed. Because the apnea episodes were associated with arterial oxygen desaturation (SpO2around 90%), a nasal probe was inserted, and oxygen, 2 l/min, was administered. The apnea episodes became less frequent and shorter at 2 h, 50 min after the administration of clonidine. No apnea episode occurred after 3 h, 5 min, and the respiratory frequency was 10–15 breaths/min. The administration of oxygen was discontinued after 3 h, 15 min. Since the beginning of oxygen administration and after its discontinuation, SpO2remained in the range of 95–99%. The end-tidal carbon dioxide was 4.7–5.4 vol% during the experiment (i.e., until 2 h after injection) and reached the maximum value of 6.5 vol% after 2 h, 55 min (while deeply sedated). The sedation score was 8.5–9.3 cm during the 120-min study period. After 4 h, 40 min, the subject could stand up and walk and was discharged shortly thereafter.

SpO2values less than 94%(92–93%) were observed in other four volunteers, three after clonidine administration and one after epinephrine administration. These episodes were short-lasting and did not require the administration of oxygen.

Epinephrine 

This is the first study showing that epidural epinephrine produces per se segmental hypoalgesia (Figure 3, top). Epinephrine suppresses noxiously evoked activity of dynamic range neurons in the spinal cord of the cat when administered intrathecally. [8] Intrathecal administration of epinephrine and other alpha-adrenergic agonists in rats produces antinociception, which can be antagonized in a competitive fashion by alpha- but not by beta-antagonists. [7] Epinephrine and norepinephrine produce a hyperpolarization of primary afferent terminals in the isolated frog spinal cord, which is antagonized by alpha2- but not by alpha1- or beta-antagonists. [28] Thus, absorption into the cerebrospinal fluid and binding to alpha2-adrenoreceptors [29] is the most likely mechanism explaining epinephrine-induced segmental hypoalgesia after epidural administration.

Segmental hypoalgesia was observed only with pinprick and only in 40% of subjects. No effect was found with the other tests used. Thus, the analgesic effect of epidural epinephrine, 100 micro gram, appears modest, and can only be detected by a weak painful stimulus, such as pinprick. Systemic absorption [10] and metabolism by spinal meningeal catechol-O-methyl transferase [11] may limit the spinal bioavailability of epidurally administered epinephrine. Further, epinephrine binds to an equal extent to the alpha2- and alpha1-adrenoreceptors, whereas clonidine has a selectivity ratio alpha2/alpha1of 200:1. [30] Hypoalgesia to pinprick could not be detected in a relatively high proportion of subjects. This may be the result of factors preventing epinephrine from reaching the alpha2-adrenoreceptors in an adequate concentration, hyposensitivity to alpha sub 2 -adrenergic agonists, poor sensitivity of the pinprick test, or inadequate dose. Because the antinociceptive effect of intrathecal epinephrine is dose-dependent in animals, [7,8] it is conceivable that the use of higher epidural doses might have been more effective.

The only side effect associated with epinephrine was short-lasting pressure pain during injection. The increase in heart rate, compared with placebo (Figure 6), was clinically not significant.

Clonidine 

Our findings confirm that analgesia after epidural clonidine is mainly the result of a spinal, rather than systemic, action. [21] Segmental analgesia or hyposensitivity was found with all tests used. Epidurally administered clonidine is rapidly absorbed into the cerebrospinal fluid, and the analgesic effect of clonidine correlates with its cerebrospinal fluid concentration. [21] If absorption into the cerebrospinal fluid with subsequent binding to the alpha2-adrenoreceptors in the spinal cord was the only mechanism explaining the analgesic effect of epidural clonidine, then analgesia at S1 after lumbar administration would be expected to be at least as good as at L1-L4. However, the quality of analgesia was better at L1 and L4 than at S1 (Figure 2, top). The dermatomes L1-L4 correspond to a spinal level close to the site of injection, wherein the concentration of clonidine in the epidural space is likely to be higher than at the more cranial and caudal segments. This suggests that inhibition of nerve conduction [31] at the nerve roots may be an important mechanism explaining clonidine epidural analgesia. The segmental hyposensitivity to cold suggests that epidural clonidine may affect nerve conduction in A delta-fibers. The wider diameter of S1 nerve roots [32] may be an additional explanation for the lesser effect of clonidine found at S1.

The decrease in pain rating after electrical stimulation at S1 (small diameter electrodes) was not significant (Figure 2, top). In contrast, a significant increase in pressure pain tolerance threshold (Figure 4, top) and in pain thresholds to electrical stimulation of the sural nerve (wider diameter electrodes)(Figure 5) were observed at the same dermatome. These findings suggest that clonidine is less effective in inhibiting short-lasting painful stimuli applied to a small area than long-lasting ones applied to a large area. This may have prevented us from detecting an analgesic effect of clonidine at dermatomes cranial and caudal to L1-L4 using electrical stimulation with small-diameter electrodes.

Epidural clonidine increased subjective and nociceptive reflex threshold to single and repeated stimulation of the sural nerve (Figure 5). Repeated stimulation of the sural nerve is a noninvasive method to investigate temporal summation in humans. [25] Nociceptive reflex thresholds to repeated stimulation are increased by isoflurane concentrations used for surgical analgesia, [33] ketamine, [34] alfentanil, [35] epidural, [36] and spinal [37] anesthesia. Repeated stimulation may lead to increased and prolonged firing of dorsal horn neurons [22](central sensitization). Spinal hyperexcitability also develops after peripheral nerve lesions [38] and is thought to be an important mechanism for neuropathic pain. [23] The inhibition of temporal summation that we have observed suggests that epidural clonidine may contribute to prevent spinal hyperexcitability (preemptive analgesia) and may explain pain relief provided by epidural or intrathecal clonidine in patients with neuropathic pain. [17–20,39] The electrophysiologic threshold after repeated stimulation is normally close to the psychophysical threshold. [25] After clonidine, the former was increased to a significantly larger extent than the latter (Figure 5). This may be the result of the aforementioned effect of clonidine on nerve conduction, [31] potentially affecting the motor component of the nociceptive reflex. Further, catecholamines depolarize the motoneurones and depress mono- and polysynaptic reflex discharges through an alpha-receptor in the spinal cord. [40] High doses of norepinephrine administered intrathecally in rats produce marked muscle weakness. [7] These evidences suggest that alpha-agonists may subclinically affect motor function after therapeutic doses.

Our results confirm the well-known effect of epidural clonidine on arterial blood pressure and heart rate. The low values of blood pressure occasionally observed may be dangerous and may require treatment in elderly or hypertensive patients or in patients with coronary artery disease. The sedative effect of epidural clonidine is also well known and is thought to be mediated by alpha2-adrenoreceptors in the locus ceruleus. [41] The use of clonidine was associated with significantly higher end-tidal carbon dioxide concentrations and lower SpO2compared with placebo. Although these effects were quantitatively modest (Figure 6), these findings and the case of severe respiratory depression suggest that patients should be strictly controlled and possibly monitored with at least pulse oximetry. All side effects may be less likely in the presence of pain, which may increase sympathetic output to heart and vessels, and counteract clonidine-induced sedation and respiratory depression.

Epidural epinephrine produces segmental hypoalgesia, which, in addition to its vasoconstrictive properties, provides a rationale for using epinephrine with local anesthetics or opioids for enhancing epidural analgesia.

Epidural clonidine produces segmental analgesia after bolus administration. The deepest effect is observed at the dermatomes corresponding to the site of injection. Clonidine should be administered at a segmental level corresponding to the painful area. Clonidine seems to be more effective in inhibiting long-lasting painful stimuli, applied to large areas, than short-lasting ones, applied to very small areas. Clonidine inhibits temporal summation elicited by repeated electrical stimulation and may contribute to prevent spinal cord hyperexcitability.

Clonidine-induced hemodynamic and respiratory effects may be a concern, particularly in patients with pre- or coexisting cardiovascular or respiratory diseases.

The authors thank Dr. Christoph Minder, statistician at the Institute of Social and Preventive Medicine of the University of Bern, for performing the statistical analyses.

1.
Yaksh TL: Pharmacology of spinal adrenergic systems which modulate spinal nociceptive processing. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1985; 22:845-58.
2.
Laishley RS, Morgan BM: A single dose epidural technique for cesarean section. Anaesthesia 1988; 43:100-3.
3.
Curatolo M, Orlando A, Zbinden AM, Scaramozzino P, Venuti FS: A multifactorial analysis to explain inadequate surgical analgesia following extradural block. Br J Anaesth 1995; 75:274-81.
4.
Bromage PR, Camporesi EM, Durant PA, Nielsen CH: Influence of epinephrine as an adjuvant to epidural morphine. Anesthesiology 1983; 58:257-62.
5.
Bromage PR, El-Faqih S, Husain I, Naguib M: Epinephrine and fentanyl as adjuvants to 0.5% bupivacaine for epidural analgesia. Reg Anesth 1989; 14:189-94.
6.
McLintic AJ, Danskin FH, Reid JA, Thorburn J: Effect of adrenaline on extradural anaesthesia, plasma lignocaine concentrations and the feto-placental unit during elective caesarean section. Br J Anaesth 1991; 67:683-9.
7.
Reddy SV, Maderdrut JL, Yaksh TL: Spinal cord pharmacology of adrenergic agonist-mediated antinociception. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1980; 213:525-33.
8.
Collins JG, Kitahata LM, Matsumoto M, Homma E, Suzukava M: Spinally administered epinephrine suppresses noxiously evoked activity of WDR neurons in the dorsal horn of the spinal cord. Anesthesiology 1984; 60:269-75.
9.
Priddle HD, Andros GJ: Primary spinal anesthetic effects of epinephrine. Anesth Analg 1950; 29:156-62.
10.
Ramanathan S, Desai NS, Zakowski M: Systemic vascular uptake of epinephrine from the lumbar epidural space in parturients. Reg Anesth 1995; 20:199-205.
11.
Kern C, Mautz DS, Bernards CM: Epinephrine is metabolized by the spinal meninges of monkeys and pigs. Anesthesiology 1995; 83:1078-81.
12.
Eisenach JC, Lysak SZ, Viscomi CM: Epidural clonidine analgesia following surgery: Phase I. Anesthesiology 1989; 71:640-6.
13.
Narchi P, Benhamou D, Hamza J, Bouaziz H: Ventilatory effects of epidural clonidine during the first 3 hours after caesarean section. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand 1992; 36:791-5.
14.
Rockemann MG, Seeling W, Brinkmann A, Goertz AW, Hauber N, Junge J, Georgieff M: Analgesic and hemodynamic effects of epidural clonidine, clonidine/morphine, and morphine after pancreatic surgery-A double blind study. Anesth Analg 1995; 80:869-74.
15.
Bernard JM, Kick O, Bonnet F: Comparison of intravenous and epidural clonidine for postoperative patient-controlled analgesia. Anesth Analg 1995; 81:706-12.
16.
De Kock M, Famenne F, Deckers G, Scholtes J-L: Epidural clonidine or sufentanil for intraoperative and postoperative analgesia. Anesth Analg 1995; 81:1154-62.
17.
Tamsen A, Gordh T: Epidural clonidine produces analgesia. Lancet 1984; 2:231-2.
18.
Glynn CJ, Jamous MA, Teddy PJ, Moore RA, Lloyd JW: Role of spinal noradrenergic system in transmission of pain in patients with spinal cord injury. Lancet 1986; 2:1249-50.
19.
Eisenach JC, DuPen S, Dubois M, Miguel R, Allin D: Epidural clonidine analgesia for intractable cancer pain. Pain 1995; 61:391-9.
20.
Glynn C, O'Sullivan K: A double-blind randomised comparison of the effects of epidural clonidine, lignocaine and the combination of clonidine and lignocaine in patients with chronic pain. Pain 1995; 64:337-43.
21.
Eisenach J, Detweiler D, Hood D: Hemodynamic and analgesic actions of epidurally administered clonidine. Anesthesiology 1993; 78:277-87.
22.
Price DD: Characteristics of second pain and flexion reflexes indicative of prolonged central summation. Exp Neurol 1972; 37:371-87.
23.
Coderre TJ, Katz J, Vaccarino AL, Melzack R: Contribution of central neuroplasticity to pathological pain: Review of clinical and experimental evidence. Pain 1993; 52:259-85.
24.
Brennum J, Kjeldsen M, Jensen K, Jensen TS: Measurements of human pressure-pain thresholds on fingers and toes. Pain 1989; 38:211-7.
25.
Arendt-Nielsen L, Brennum J, Sindrup S, Bak P: Electrophysiological and psychophysical quantification of central temporal summation of the human nociceptive system. Eur J Appl Physiol 1994; 68:266-73.
26.
Altman DG: Practical Statistics for Medical Research. London, UK, Chapman and Hall, 1991, pp 325-64.
27.
SAS Institute Inc.: Repeated Measures Analysis of Variance, SASR Systems for Linear Models. Edited by SAS Institute Inc. Cary, NC, USA, SAS Institute Inc., 1991, pp 265-319.
28.
Wohlberg CJ, Hackman JC, Ryan GP, Davidoff RA: Epinephrine-and norepinephrine-evoked potential changes of frog primary afferent terminals: Pharmacological characterization of alpha and beta components. Brain Res 1985; 327:289-301.
29.
Nukina I, La Bella FS: Agonist-induced regulation of adrenoreceptor subtypes in cerebral cortical slices. J Neurochem 1987; 49:389-92.
30.
Maze M, Tranquilli W: Alpha-2 adrenoreceptor agonists: Defining the role in clinical anesthesia. Anesthesiology 1991; 74:581-605.
31.
Butterworth JF, Strichartz GR: The alpha sub 2 -adrenergic agonists clonidine and guanfacine produce tonic and phasic block of conduction in rat sciatic nerve fibers. Anesth Analg 1993; 76:295-301.
32.
Hogan Q: Size of human lower thoracic and lumbosacral nerve roots. Anesthesiology 1996; 85:37-42.
33.
Petersen-Felix S, Arendt-Nielsen L, Bak P, Fischer M, Bjerring P, Zbinden AM: The effects of isoflurane on repeated nociceptive stimuli (central temporal summation). Pain 1995; 64:277-81.
34.
Arendt-Nielsen L, Petersen-Felix S, Fischer M, Bak P, Bjerring P, Zbinden AM: The effect of N-methyl-D-aspartate antagonist (ketamine) on single and repeated nociceptive stimuli: A placebo-controlled experimental human study. Anesth Analg 1995; 81:63-8.
35.
Petersen-Felix S, Arendt-Nielsen L, Bak P, Fischer M, Zbinden AM: Psychophysical and electrophysiological responses to experimental pain may be influenced by sedation: Comparison of the effects of an hypnotic (propofol) and an analgesic (alfentanil). Br J Anaesth 1996; 77:165-71.
36.
Curatolo M, Petersen-Felix S, Arendt-Nielsen L, Fischer M, Zbinden AM: Temporal summation during extradural anaesthesia. Br J Anaesth 1995; 75:634-5.
37.
Curatolo M, Petersen-Felix S, Arendt-Nielsen L, Zbinden AM: Spinal anaesthesia inhibits central temporal summation. Br J Anaesth 1997; 78:88-9.
38.
Luo L, Puke MJC, Wiesenfeld-Hallin Z: The effects of intrathecal morphine and clonidine on the prevention and reversal of spinal cord hyperexcitability following sciatic nerve section in the rat. Pain 1994; 58:245-52.
39.
Siddal PJ, Gray M, Rutkowski S, Cousins MJ: Intrathecal morphine and clonidine in the management of spinal cord injury pain: A case report. Pain 1994; 59:147-8.
40.
Kitazawa T, Saito K, Ohga A: Effects of catecholamines on spinal motoneurones and spinal reflex discharges in the isolated spinal cord of the newborn rat. Brain Res 1985; 351:31-6.
41.
De Sarro GB, Ascioti C, Froio F, Libri V, Nistico G: Evidence that locus coeruleus is the site where clonidine and drugs acting at alpha sub 1 - and alpha sub 2 -adrenoreceptors affect sleep and arousal mechanisms. Br J Pharmacol 1987; 90:675-85.