Background

Residency training requires work in clinical settings for extended periods of time, resulting in altered sleep patterns, sleep deprivation, and potentially deleterious effects on safe performance of daily activities, including driving a motor vehicle.

Methods

Twenty-nine anesthesiology resident physicians in postgraduate year 2 to 4 drove for 55 min in the Virginia Driving Safety Laboratory using the Driver Guidance System (MBFARR, LLC, USA). Two driving simulator sessions were conducted, one experimental session immediately after the final shift of six consecutive night shifts and one control session at the beginning of a normal day shift (not after call). Both sessions were conducted at 8:00 am. Psychomotor vigilance task testing was employed to evaluate reaction time and lapses in attention.

Results

After six consecutive night shifts, residents experienced significantly impaired control of all the driving variables including speed, lane position, throttle, and steering. They were also more likely to be involved in collisions. After six consecutive night shifts, residents had a significant increase in reaction times (281.1 vs. 298.5 ms; P = 0.001) and had a significant increase in the number of both minor (0.85 vs. 1.88; P = 0.01) and major lapses (0.00 vs. 0.31; P = 0.008) in attention.

Conclusions

Resident physicians have greater difficulty controlling speed and driving performance in the driving simulator after six consecutive night shifts. Reaction times are also increased with emphasis on increases in minor and major lapses in attention after six consecutive night shifts.

Resident physicians have greater difficulty controlling speed and driving performance in the driving simulator after six consecutive night shifts. Reaction times are also increased with emphasis on increases in minor and major lapses in attention after six consecutive night shifts.

What We Already Know about This Topic
  • Residency training requires work in clinical settings for extended periods of time, resulting in altered sleep patterns, sleep deprivation, and potentially deleterious effects on safe performance of daily activities.

  • This study determined driving performance of residents after six consecutive overnight work shifts.

What This Article Tells Us That Is New
  • Resident physicians have greater difficulty controlling speed and driving performance in the driving simulator after six consecutive night shifts. Reaction times are also increased with emphasis on increases in minor and major lapses in attention after six consecutive night shifts.

The Accreditation Council for Graduate Medical Education (ACGME) initiated duty hour limitations in 20031  and again in 20112  to promote high-quality learning and safe care. Despite these changes, residency training continues to require extended work in hospital and clinic settings, including overnight shifts. This results in altered sleep patterns, sleep deprivation, and potentially deleterious effects on the safe performance of daily activities. Before the initiation of duty hour restrictions in 2003, a survey of 2,734 interns determined that there was an increased likelihood of motor vehicle accidents and near-miss incidents after a single extended (approximately 32 h) work shift.3  In that same time period before duty hour implementation, a single study of driving simulator performance among internal medicine residents and medical students after one night on call demonstrated impaired lane position variance and increased number of crashes.4  Some studies in nonhealthcare professionals have evaluated the effects of acute sleep deprivation on performance in a driving simulator.5–7  We sought to rigorously study the impact of six consecutive overnight work shifts on resident physician driving performance in a high-fidelity driving simulator,8  rather than screen-based simulation employed in other studies.9 

Sleep deprivation has neurobehavioral consequences and deleterious effects on cognitive functions such as vigilance, selective attention, orientation to sensory events, and attention to executive processing and tasks.10,11  These effects are measurable through tests that focus on deterioration of attention such as psychomotor vigilance task (PVT) testing. PVT testing is highly sensitive to both acute and chronic sleep deprivation and measures reaction time to visual stimuli that occur at random intervals as well as lapses in attention.12,13  As such, PVT testing has been used to demonstrate the effects of sleep loss from jet lag and shift work.14  Indeed, PVT performance possesses implications for real-world risks, as deficits in sustained attention (lapses) and timely reactions adversely affect tasks in which response is critical, such as safe driving performance.11 

We hypothesize that after six consecutive overnight work shifts, anesthesiology residents will have poorer driving performance as evidenced by impaired control of speed, lane position, throttle, and steering and will be involved in more collisions compared to their day shift work state. As concurrent validation of impairment, we further hypothesize that they will have impaired reaction time and more lapses on PVT testing when compared with their day shift work state.

After approval by the Institutional Review Board (University of Virginia Health System, Charlottesville, Virginia), anesthesiology residents on a prescheduled night float rotation at the University of Virginia were offered the opportunity to participate. From March 21, 2014 to February 11, 2015, 30 consecutive anesthesiology resident physicians gave written informed consent to participate in the study, and no residents declined participation. Two simulator sessions were conducted and both occurred at 8:00 am, with the experimental session immediately after the final shift of six consecutive night shifts and one control session at the beginning of a normal day shift (not after call). A minimum of 10 days elapsed between the experimental and control sessions. This “night float” system is in compliance with current ACGME duty hour requirements, and there were no duty hour violations during the period of the study. At each evaluation session, residents provided information about their sleep habits and the use of any sleep aids during the previous week and completed an Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS) before and after each driving simulation session. For reference, the ESS is included (appendix 1) and requires respondents to rate their chance of dozing off to sleep in eight different situations, scored from 0 (never doze off) to 3 (high chance to doze off). ESS scores from 0 to 10 are considered normal, scores from 10 to 12 are borderline sleepiness and scores from 12 to 24 are abnormal, with high risk to doze. The ESS was employed as a validated, reproducible measure of resident sleepiness.15 

Before each simulated driving experience, all residents underwent PVT testing. PVT is a 10-min computerized test that evaluates sustained attention and reaction time and measures lapses, both minor (less than 0.5 s) and major (0.5 to 1.0 s) in response, after randomly occurring visual stimuli.16–18  After an opportunity to practice the PVT, residents sat in a quiet, well-lit room with a laptop on the desk in front of them and were instructed to push the space bar when the visual prompt appeared on the screen. PVT testing is independent of aptitude and learning effects, but reliable and sensitive to performance variation due to fatigue.19 

The Driver Guidance System (fig. 1) is a high-fidelity immersive driving simulator made by MBFARR, LLC. Forward visuals are produced on a 210° curved screen by three projectors, along with virtual side and rear-view mirrors. The driving station has an adjustable seat, force feedback steering, digital dashboard, gear selector, brake, and gas pedals. A customized driving scenario was developed, utilizing an oval track with four lanes. The simulator room is controlled for light and sound.

Residents were seated and had time to familiarize themselves with the controls of the Driver Guidance System and were given instructions to drive the 2-mile, 4-lane oval track in the center of their current lane, to maintain speed at 45 mph, and to avoid obstacles when they appeared. The driving course was divided into four epochs, in order to investigate how quickly impaired driving could be detected: epoch 1 was 10 min of open road driving; epochs 2 to 4 were 15 min each of open road driving with nine randomly appearing obstacles. In total, residents drove for 55 min and were required to avoid collision with all obstacles. There was no other simulated traffic or distractions. We have previously demonstrated that high-fidelity driving simulation has high test–retest reliability (r = 0.8, P < 0.001) and robust internal consistency (P < 0.001) with minimal practice effect in multiple settings, including adults with diabetes mellitus, military personnel with history of traumatic brain injury, and young adults with attention deficit disorder and risky teenage behaviors as evaluated in a high-fidelity virtual driving simulator.20–24 

Statistical Analysis

A power analysis was not conducted before enrolling resident physicians as this was a purely exploratory study. Based on our experience and our knowledge of resident night float clinical responsibilities, we aimed to enroll as many residents as possible. Five outcome variables in the driving simulator were measured, including speed, lane position, throttle, steering, and the number of collisions with obstacles (0 to 9). Steering and speed control are the most vulnerable measures of sleep deprivation. Since the resident interacted with the high-fidelity driving simulator only through turning of the steering wheel and control of the foot pedals, our performance variables were restricted to speed, steering, and collisions. A recent investigation has reported that poor speed control is a robust predictor of future collisions among novice drivers.25  In the open road condition, 2 × 4 repeated measures ANOVA was conducted to evaluate the impact of day versus night shift performance on all of the variables. For the obstacle segments, 2 × 3 repeated measures ANOVA was conducted as the first epoch was removed, as it was entirely open road driving. Descriptive data from the ESS, sleepiness questionnaire, sleep habits log, and PVT testing data were collected and analyzed with the paired t test. Statistical software analysis was completed with IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 22.0. (IBM Corp., USA).

Descriptive data from 29 residents are presented in table 1. One resident was consented for the study but was unable to complete the questionnaires, PVT, or driving simulation. In the night shift state, residents slept fewer hours daily in the previous week (6.6 ± 1.13 vs. 7.2 ± 0.74; P = 0.002) as well as in the 24 h before the driving simulation (6.3 ± 1.46 vs. 7.2 ± 1.1; P = 0.025). In the night shift state, residents also had significantly increased sleepiness scale scores (ESS night shift predrive score 10.2 ± 4.99 vs. day shift predrive score 6.4 ± 3.84; P < 0.001 and ESS night shift postdrive score 12.3 ± 5.66 vs. day shift postdrive score 7.3 ± 3.9; P < 0.001). On the sleepiness questionnaire (appendix 2), residents reported feeling less alert and less safe to drive in the night shift as compared to the day shift state.

Data from 26 of the 29 residents were suitable for driving simulator analysis. Data from three residents were lost after a hard drive failure and could not be analyzed. Figure 2 and table 2 display driving performance in the open road condition. Residents in the night shift work state had difficulty in controlling speed (P < 0.001), lane position (P = 0.03), throttle (P < 0.001), and steering (P = 0.01) compared to day shift condition. Figure 3 and table 3 illustrate driving performance in the obstacle segments during 2 s before and 6 s after the appearance of an obstacle. In order to avoid a collision, residents in the night shift state had greater mean deviation in speed (P = 0.04) and throttle (P = 0.04), whereas steering (P = 0.73) and lane position (P = 0.80) were not significantly different compared to their day shift work condition. Residents in the night shift work state also had increased number of collisions compared to day shift residents (epoch 2: 0.75 vs. 0.3; epoch 3: 0.45 vs. 0.23; and epoch 4: 0.30 vs. 0.15; P = 0.010).

Twenty-six residents’ PVT data were analyzed for comparison of mean reaction time as well as the number of major and minor lapses in the night shift versus day shift states. Day shift and night shift conditions differed in PVT in reaction times (281.1 ± 33.1 ms vs. 298.5 ± 32.1 ms; P = 0.001), minor lapses less than 0.5 s (0.85 ± 1.32 vs. 1.88 ± 2.18; P = 0.01), and major lapses greater than 0.5 s (0.00 ± 0 vs. 0.31 ± 0.55; P = 0.008).

After six consecutive overnight work shifts, anesthesiology residents have impaired driving performance in a high-fidelity driving simulator. Even in the open road without any obstacles to avoid, residents experienced significantly impaired control of all the driving variables including speed, lane position, throttle, and steering (fig. 2) after six consecutive night shifts. While residents in the night shift condition seemed to have consistently poor control of speed and lane position, especially at the beginning of their driving, residents in the day shift condition initially started out with better control but experienced degradation of control of speed and lane position over time. We speculate that this may represent boredom or task fatigue over the course of performing a monotonous activity. Indeed, informal comments from residents support this hypothesis. Figure 3 displays similar impairment of resident driving performance, with residents in the night shift condition swerving to avoid collision with each obstacle.

On PVT testing, there was a small but significant difference (17 ms) in reaction times between the day shift and night shift conditions. A change in reaction time of 17 ms, while statistically significant, is unlikely to be relevant or clinically important in the real world. Most importantly, we found significant differences via the PVT in the number of minor (0.2 to 0.5 s) and major (greater than 0.5 s) lapses. We believe that the increased number of lapses in the night shift condition is much more important than the change in mean reaction time because it is these lapses in attention that may lead to collisions and accidents.

Although driving simulator performance may be more sensitive to the effects of fatigue compared to real road driving, high-fidelity driving simulators are considered to be a valid measure of driving performance.26  Using a high-fidelity driving simulator, internal medicine residents and medical students demonstrated increased lane position variability and increased number of crashes after a single night on call.4  Using a single-monitor computer simulator, a study in pediatric residents revealed poor driving performance after overnight call during a “heavy-call” (every fourth or fifth night) rotation.9 

Before the implementation of resident duty hour restrictions in 2003, Barger et al.3  demonstrated that there was an increased likelihood of motor vehicle accidents and near-miss incidents after an extended work shift (mean duration of 32 h). Using a screen-based (nonimmersive) simulator, Arnedt et al.9  found that the postcall performance driving impairment during a heavy call rotation was comparable to the level of impairment associated with 0.04 to 0.05 g% blood alcohol. Although the degree to which simulator results can be transferred to real road driving is unknown, our results suggest that driving performance is impaired even after a 14-h night shift, consistent with the most current ACGME duty hour standards. Indeed, night shift residents in this study slept 6.33 h on average in the 24 h before the driving experiment. These results suggest that changing from a traditional call system to a night-float or night shift system does not protect residents from the impact of fatigue on driving performance, even though it may allow residents more sleep during the day.

Our evaluation of driving performance in a simulator after night shift is not without limitations. One limitation is that residents were always enrolled in the study as they began their night shift week and thus the night shift condition was always evaluated before the control, day shift state. This may cause either the early epochs of the night shift sessions to appear worse or the day shift sessions to be artificially improved due to a possible “simulator learning” effect. We believe that the potential impact of any “simulator learning” effect is minimal as driving is not a new skill to all the residents in the study, and other studies our group conducted using high-fidelity driving simulation have not demonstrated a learning effect.8,20–24  Indeed, we have previously demonstrated that high-fidelity driving simulation has high test–retest reliability (r = 0.8, P < 0.001) with robust internal consistency (P < 0.01) on five operational tests with no significant learning effect noted with mean change of 0.86 and 1.35 points on operational and tactical driving quotients (operational tests: visual, motor, and cognitive abilities required for safe operation of motor vehicle; tactical tests: steering, braking, speed control, and judgment variables), respectively.8,20–24  Finally, the applicability of any study conducted in a simulator—even high-fidelity immersive simulation—to real-life driving is unknown.

Night work is an unavoidable aspect of medicine. Anesthesiology residency programs limit night float work in order to regulate duty hours and to provide time for adequate resident rest. Unfortunately, there is a paucity of information regarding the optimal duration of night float work and the impact of various “call” strategies on resident well-being. The goal of future investigations will be focused on answering these critically important questions. As a specialty, we must endeavor to train the next generation of physician anesthesiologists to be the best possible clinicians and scientists; however, we must also craft our training to maximize the personal well-being and safety of our trainees. We call upon other investigators to help us answer these important questions.

Residents have greater difficulty controlling speed and other measures of driving performance in the driving simulator, including obstacle collisions, after six consecutive overnight work shifts. In addition, reaction times are increased with more major and minor lapses of attention.

Support was provided solely from institutional and/or departmental sources.

The authors declare no competing interests.

1.
Philibert
I
,
Friedmann
P
,
Williams
WT
;
ACGME Work Group on Resident Duty Hours. Accreditation Council for Graduate Medical Education
ACGME Work Group on Resident Duty Hours. Accreditation Council for Graduate Medical Education
:
New requirements for resident duty hours.
JAMA
2002
;
288
:
1112
4
2.
Nasca
TJ
,
Day
SH
,
Amis
ES
Jr
;
ACGME Duty Hour Task Force
ACGME Duty Hour Task Force
:
The new recommendations on duty hours from the ACGME Task Force.
N Engl J Med
2010
;
363
:
e3
3.
Barger
LK
,
Cade
BE
,
Ayas
NT
,
Cronin
JW
,
Rosner
B
,
Speizer
FE
,
Czeisler
CA
;
Harvard Work Hours, Health, and Safety Group
Harvard Work Hours, Health, and Safety Group
:
Extended work shifts and the risk of motor vehicle crashes among interns.
N Engl J Med
2005
;
352
:
125
34
4.
Ware
JC
,
Risser
MR
,
Manser
T
,
Karlson
KH
Jr
:
Medical resident driving simulator performance following a night on call.
Behav Sleep Med
2006
;
4
:
1
12
5.
Jackson
ML
,
Croft
RJ
,
Kennedy
GA
,
Owens
K
,
Howard
ME
:
Cognitive components of simulated driving performance: Sleep loss effects and predictors.
Accid Anal Prev
2013
;
50
:
438
44
6.
Neubauer
C
,
Matthews
G
,
Langheim
L
,
Saxby
D
:
Fatigue and voluntary utilization of automation in simulated driving.
Hum Factors
2012
;
54
:
734
46
7.
Akerstedt
T
,
Peters
B
,
Anund
A
,
Kecklund
G
:
Impaired alertness and performance driving home from the night shift: A driving simulator study.
J Sleep Res
2005
;
14
:
17
20
8.
Cox
SM
,
Cox
DJ
,
Kofler
MJ
,
Moncrief
MA
,
Johnson
RJ
,
Lambert
AE
,
Cain
SA
,
Reeve
RE
:
Driving simulator performance in novice drivers with autism spectrum disorder: The role of executive functions and basic motor skills.
J Autism Dev Disord
2016
;
46
:
1379
91
9.
Arnedt
JT
,
Owens
J
,
Crouch
M
,
Stahl
J
,
Carskadon
MA
:
Neurobehavioral performance of residents after heavy night call vs after alcohol ingestion.
JAMA
2005
;
294
:
1025
33
10.
Banks
S
,
Dinges
DF
:
Behavioral and physiological consequences of sleep restriction.
J Clin Sleep Med
2007
;
3
:
519
28
11.
Basner
M
,
Dinges
DF
:
Maximizing sensitivity of the psychomotor vigilance test (PVT) to sleep loss.
Sleep
2011
;
34
:
581
91
12.
Lim
J
,
Dinges
DF
:
Sleep deprivation and vigilant attention.
Ann N Y Acad Sci
2008
;
1129
:
305
22
13.
Dorrian
J
,
Rogers
NL
,
Dinges
DF
,
Kushida
CA
:
Psychomotor vigilance performance: Neurocognitive assay sensitive to sleep loss
in
Sleep Deprivation: Clinical Issues, Pharmacology, and Sleep Loss Effects
.
Edited by Kushida CA. New York
,
Marcel Dekker, Inc.
,
2005
, pp
39
70
14.
Neri
DF
,
Oyung
RL
,
Colletti
LM
,
Mallis
MM
,
Tam
PY
,
Dinges
DF
:
Controlled breaks as a fatigue countermeasure on the flight deck.
Aviat Space Environ Med
2002
;
73
:
654
64
15.
Johns
MW
:
A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: The Epworth sleepiness scale.
Sleep
1991
;
14
:
540
5
16.
Dinges
DF
,
Powell
JW
:
Microcomputer analyses of performance on a portable, simple visual RT task during sustained operations.
Behav Res Methods
1985
;
17
:
652
5
17.
Jewett
ME
,
Kronauer
RE
:
Interactive mathematical models of subjective alertness and cognitive throughput in humans.
J Biol Rhythms
1999
;
14
:
588
97
18.
Khitrov
MY
,
Laxminarayan
S
,
Thorsley
D
,
Ramakrishnan
S
,
Rajaraman
S
,
Wesensten
NJ
,
Reifman
J
:
PC-PVT: A platform for psychomotor vigilance task testing, analysis, and prediction.
Behav Res Methods
2014
;
46
:
140
7
19.
Doran
SM
,
Van Dongen
HP
,
Dinges
DF
:
Sustained attention performance during sleep deprivation: Evidence of state instability.
Arch Ital Biol
2001
;
139
:
253
67
20.
Cox
DJ
,
Gonder-Frederick
LA
,
Kovatchev
BP
,
Julian
DM
,
Clarke
WL
:
Progressive hypoglycemia’s impact on driving simulation performance. Occurrence, awareness and correction.
Diabetes Care
2000
;
23
:
163
70
21.
Cox
DJ
,
Davis
M
,
Singh
H
,
Barbour
B
,
Nidiffer
FD
,
Trudel
T
,
Mourant
R
,
Moncrief
R
:
Driving rehabilitation for military personnel recovering from traumatic brain injury using virtual reality driving simulation: A feasibility study.
Mil Med
2010
;
175
:
411
6
22.
Cox
DJ
,
Punja
M
,
Powers
K
,
Merkel
RL
,
Burket
R
,
Moore
M
,
Thorndike
F
,
Kovatchev
B
:
Manual transmission enhances attention and driving performance of ADHD adolescent males: Pilot study.
J Atten Disord
2006
;
10
:
212
6
23.
Cox
DJ
,
Madaan
V
,
Cox
BS
:
Adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and driving: Why and how to manage it.
Curr Psychiatry Rep
2011
;
13
:
345
50
24.
Cox
DJ
,
Banton
T
,
Record
S
,
Grabman
JH
,
Hawkins
RJ
:
Does correcting astigmatism with toric lenses improve driving performance?
Optom Vis Sci
2015
;
92
:
404
11
25.
Cox
DJ
,
Moncrief
M
,
Rizzo
M
,
Fisher
D
,
Lambert
A
,
Thomas
S
,
Moncrief
R
:
Low hanging fruit: Use of virtual reality simulation in Department of Motor Vehicles to assess minimal competence of novice drivers
in
8th International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driving Assessment, Training, and Vehicle Design
26.
Hallvig
D
,
Anund
A
,
Fors
C
,
Kecklund
G
,
Karlsson
JG
,
Wahde
M
,
Akerstedt
T
:
Sleepy driving on the real road and in the simulator—A comparison.
Accid Anal Prev
2013
;
50
:
44
50

Appendix 1. Epworth Sleepiness Scale

Appendix 2. Sleepiness and Driving Questionnaire

Competing Interests

This article is featured in “This Month in Anesthesiology,” page 1A.